首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   4662篇
  免费   243篇
  国内免费   11篇
  2023年   49篇
  2022年   49篇
  2021年   52篇
  2020年   118篇
  2019年   120篇
  2018年   168篇
  2017年   163篇
  2016年   171篇
  2015年   109篇
  2014年   146篇
  2013年   507篇
  2012年   214篇
  2011年   228篇
  2010年   169篇
  2009年   152篇
  2008年   188篇
  2007年   213篇
  2006年   191篇
  2005年   161篇
  2004年   146篇
  2003年   146篇
  2002年   137篇
  2001年   102篇
  2000年   64篇
  1999年   65篇
  1998年   52篇
  1997年   46篇
  1996年   45篇
  1995年   55篇
  1994年   62篇
  1993年   43篇
  1992年   58篇
  1991年   42篇
  1990年   39篇
  1989年   36篇
  1988年   34篇
  1987年   34篇
  1986年   34篇
  1985年   38篇
  1984年   38篇
  1983年   33篇
  1982年   32篇
  1981年   26篇
  1980年   28篇
  1979年   35篇
  1978年   43篇
  1977年   35篇
  1976年   22篇
  1974年   27篇
  1973年   27篇
排序方式: 共有4916条查询结果,搜索用时 140 毫秒
971.
Sound effects were inserted in a television program in order to guide children's selective attention to, and comprehension of, significant story content. Sixty-four children, equally distributed by sex and by kindergarten and fifth grades, were randomly assigned to one of four treatment conditions that crossed two levels of sound effects with two levels of visual inserts. One-s sound effects either preceded or did not precede three key program transitions. The visual insert conditions provided additional information at these program points whereas the no visual insert conditions did not. Visual attention was videotaped during each child's individual viewing session. After viewing, each child answered a 22-item multiple-choice recognition test of inferential, central, and incedental content. As predicted, sound effects increased selective attention and inferential recognition, particularly for the youngest children who have the greatest difficulty understanding televised stories.  相似文献   
972.
Some families present with so many problems and disordered family processes that where and how to begin is an extremely important question. When premature termination of therapy also appears likely, a family therapist may simply attempt to initiate a structural change within the family system so that the family can continue on its own. This article describes the responses of a highly chaotic family to such a single therapeutic intent.  相似文献   
973.
974.
Maintenance of self-esteem and of social relationships are major tasks for cancer patients, and are facilitated by supportive interactions with a variety of others. Different sources potentially provide different forms of support. Patients value informational support from doctors, with concurrent emotional support; family members and confidants specialise in the provision of empathy, reassurance of personal worth, and encouragement; and other cancer patients may act as supplementary sources of both information and empathy. Counsellors may provide direct social support in the forms of empathy and access to material help and information, and may also facilitate socially supportive interactions between the patient and other natural providers of support such as doctors, family, and peers.  相似文献   
975.
Summary The present experiment assessed a traditional assumption regarding the informational role of knowledge of results (KR) in motor learning. The assumption is that learning is facilitated to the degree that KR is used to reduce goal-directed error. To test the assumption we examined two specific predictions that have been made with respect to post-KR interval effects: that the interval should be long enough for action-planning operations to occur and that the interval be free from interpolated activities that might prevent action-planning operations. The present study used a factorial arrangement of post-KR interval durations and interpolated activities during the post-KR interval to test these predictions. Using a movement timing task, we found that, contrary to predictions, learning was not facilitated by lengthening the post-KR interval. Also, in opposition to existing theory, the data revealed that interpolated activities, when combined with longer post-KR durations, were not detrimental to learning and actually improved learning in some instances. These findings are discussed together with recent evidence regarding various KR effects that suggest that many theoretical assumptions about the role of KR need revision.  相似文献   
976.
This article attempts a comprehensive and critical review of the by-now fairly extensive literature on the Barnum effect—the approval/acceptance by subjects of bogus personality interpretations supposedly derived from standard tests. Since the last major review eight years ago various methodological extensions have occurred and various rival hypotheses for established findings have been proposed. The present review is divided into three major sections: client and clinician characteristics; feedback statements and test format; and implications for personality assessment and measurement. Nearly 50 studies on the acceptance of personality interpretations are systematically reviewed and criticized.  相似文献   
977.
Recently, a nonparametric technique called bootstrapping has been recommended over the more well-known analysis of variance (ANOVA) for analyzing repeated measures data. Advocates cite as bootstrap’s advantages over ANOVA the fact that the former uses distributional information and is free of normal theory assumptions. The present study used a computer simulation to compare the two techniques calculated using data sampled from normal and nonnormal distributions. The parametric test had adequate control of Type I error rates; the nonparametric test had overly liberal Type I error rates and therefore is not recommended.  相似文献   
978.
979.
Diurnal variation in the Positive Affects   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
We examined the circadian rhythm of two basic dimensions of mood—Positive Affect (PA) and Negative Affect (NA). Subjects were 196 college students in two studies who completed a mood rating form approximately seven times a day for 1 week. Significant diurnal variation was found to exist in overall Positive Affect, and in four component content areas, but not in Negative Affect. Specifically, all components of PA rose sharply from early morning until noon, remained relatively constant until 9 p.m., and then fell rapidly. The rise and fall of PA were quite robust across subjects, but the apparently static level of PA from noon to 9 p.m. was shown to result from averaging across marked individual differences in the time of peak PA. Various potential sources of these individual differences—“morningness-eveningness,” introversion-extraversion, and neuroticism/dysphoria—were investigated. Neither of the personality variables accounted for the significant variation in diurnal mood patterning, but clearly defined morning and evening types did show different patterns. Possible links between the mood pattern obtained and biologically based circadian rhythms, which have been postulated to be dysregulated in depression and mania, are discussed.  相似文献   
980.
Discourse in aphasia: Integration deficits in processing reference   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
The present study investigated moderately impaired aphasic subjects' ability to integrate information across sentences by having them identify antecedents for ambiguous pronouns in brief narratives. In order to disambiguate the pronouns, the subjects had to consult either textual cues and/or extratextual cues. In addition, the subjects' ability to retrieve factual information was measured by having them identify explicitly stated noun referents. The results show that the aphasic subjects had significant difficulty using textual cues to resolve referents when the referents were not readily accessible through world knowledge. The patients had little difficulty interpreting referents which were recoverable from world knowledge or were stated explicitly. The explanatory power of world knowledge effects, grammatical class effects, memory effects, and linguistic integration effects in accounting for the deficit pattern observed is considered.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号