全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2004篇 |
免费 | 121篇 |
专业分类
2125篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 20篇 |
2021年 | 26篇 |
2020年 | 44篇 |
2019年 | 55篇 |
2018年 | 61篇 |
2017年 | 62篇 |
2016年 | 56篇 |
2015年 | 42篇 |
2014年 | 53篇 |
2013年 | 253篇 |
2012年 | 82篇 |
2011年 | 85篇 |
2010年 | 25篇 |
2009年 | 55篇 |
2008年 | 65篇 |
2007年 | 56篇 |
2006年 | 66篇 |
2005年 | 55篇 |
2004年 | 47篇 |
2003年 | 43篇 |
2002年 | 50篇 |
2001年 | 25篇 |
2000年 | 28篇 |
1999年 | 26篇 |
1998年 | 26篇 |
1997年 | 28篇 |
1996年 | 18篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 17篇 |
1991年 | 23篇 |
1990年 | 27篇 |
1989年 | 18篇 |
1988年 | 23篇 |
1987年 | 24篇 |
1986年 | 27篇 |
1985年 | 19篇 |
1984年 | 19篇 |
1983年 | 24篇 |
1982年 | 35篇 |
1981年 | 27篇 |
1980年 | 16篇 |
1979年 | 24篇 |
1978年 | 22篇 |
1977年 | 24篇 |
1976年 | 24篇 |
1975年 | 22篇 |
1974年 | 23篇 |
1973年 | 20篇 |
1968年 | 16篇 |
1962年 | 14篇 |
排序方式: 共有2125条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
Murray J. White 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1970,8(2):107-109
The effect of retinal locus on the letter-span error function was investigated by requiring Ss to fixate at the left, middle, or right of lines of letters presented for 100 msec. Stimulus presentations consisted of lines of eight letters presented across the visual field, and lines of four letters presented alternately in the left and right visual hemifields. Locus of fixation and relative letter position were found to be significantly related to whether or not a letter was correctly localized. A significant interaction between locus of fixation and letter position was observed. 相似文献
22.
M. B. Sterman J. J. Lynch M. T. Orne D. Paskewitz J. Costello N. Nicolov O. Diankov M. Popova E. Tsvetanska Robert G. Grenell J. J. Izquierdo V. H. Mark Joseph Germana N. Zill William P. Paré Joseph E. O. Newton George C. Offutt Walter Ehrlich Joseph E. D. Newton Julij Tosef J. Perez-Cruet Chester R. Wilpizeski John F. Lontz Andrew Livingston Joseph W. Cullen Samuel A. Corson Herman R. Weed Elizabeth O. Corson O. D. Murphree Paul N. Morgan Ruth Jarman J. Antal T. A. Ban J. V. Ananth H. E. Lehmann A. Ulric Moore Richard H. Barnes Wilson G. Pond 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1970,5(3):185-193
23.
Generalization gradients and stimulus control in delayed matching-to-sample 总被引:4,自引:3,他引:1 下载免费PDF全文
Murray Sidman 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1969,12(5):745-757
Neurological patients were subjects in delayed visual matching-to-sample. The sample and choice stimuli were ellipses of varying size. By measuring the difference in size between the sample on a given trial and the ellipse the subject chose on that trial, gradients of differences between samples and choice stimuli could be plotted. These difference gradients broadened with increasing delays. Sharp gradients were controlled by the samples. Flat gradients were controlled by features of the choice display, independently of the samples. Intermediate gradients reflected combined control by the samples and by the choice displays. 相似文献
24.
Murray Sidman Peter B. Rosenberger 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1967,10(5):467-478
Three keys were available for monkeys to press. The objective was to teach the animals to press the keys in sequences up to 10 members in length. With fading procedures, a light that cued the correct key at a given serial member of the sequence faded out slightly each time the animal selected it, and became slightly brighter after the animal made an error at that sequence member. The correct keys were faded out, starting from the end of the sequence and proceeding toward the beginning. With control procedures, the cue lights were turned off suddenly, rather than being faded gradually. In almost every instance, the animals learned a longer series of unlighted key positions with the fading procedures than they did when each key-light was turned off suddenly. Also, requiring the animals to press a series of keys cued by lights before they could reach the sequence members they were to learn hampered them in learning the later serial members. By using several different sequences, it was possible to replicate these findings within the individual animal. 相似文献
25.
Harold W. Hake Gerald W. Faust John S. McIntyre Harry G. Murray 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1967,2(10):469-478
Judgments were made of the size of a small, luminous (inner) square appearing in the center of a somewhat larger, variable (outer) square, the whole pattern appearing in the dark. Accuracy of judgment was greatest when the outer square was constant from trial to trial but deteriorated when the outer square also varied in size, reaching a lower limit less than the accuracy produced under the absolute judgment condition (no outer square presented). An application of multiple discriminant analysis and the use of a maximum likelihood observer model provided estimates of the extent to which Os responded to inner and outer size variation as separate aspects of stimulation. Although Os differed markedly in this respect, their ability to identify inner square size under the various conditions did not reflect this difference. 相似文献
26.
The effect of encoding training on tachistoscopic performance was measured by training groups of Ss in either an octal or a standardized English encoding of nine place binary numbers. A control group of Ss was allowed to encode ad lib during an equivalent training period. Performance was measured before, during and after the encoding training. It was found that imposing a code on S impaired tachistoscopic performance both during and for some time after the encoding training. Possible reasons for this effect are discussed. 相似文献
27.
Samuel C. McLaughlin Kenneth I. Rifkin Robert G. Webster 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1966,1(5):452-458
When S looks at a visual target through prisms, adaptive shifts in reaching behavior occur even though he sees no part of his body through the prisms. These shifts are caused by a change in the judgment of the direction of gaze (oculomotor change), which in turn is caused by two secondary prismatic effects: (a) asymmetry of the visual display and (b) apparent rotation about a vertical axis of a panel or wall facing S. The “asymmetry” factor contributes 22% of the total oculomotor change, and the “rotation” effect contributes the remaining 78%. Oculomotor change is not facilitated by eye-movement activity. The adaptive oculomotor change induces a non-adaptive proprioception change about one-tenth as large as the oculomotor change. These findings are capable of accounting for the previously unexplained results reported by Wooster in 1923, and also for the current controversy about the role of reafferent stimulation in sensorymotor adaptation. 相似文献
28.
Samuel C. McLaughlin Kenneth I. Rifkin Robert G. Webster 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1966,1(12):452-458
When S looks at a visual target through prisms, adaptive shifts in reaching behavior occur even though he sees no part of his body through the prisms. These shifts are caused by a change in the judgment of the direction of gaze (oculomotor change), which in turn is caused by two secondary prismatic effects: (a) asymmetry of the visual display and (b) apparent rotation about a vertical axis of a panel or wall facing S. The “asymmetry” factor contributes 22% of the total oculomotor change, and the “rotation” effect contributes the remaining 78%. Oculomotor change is not facilitated by eye-movzment activity. The adaptive oculomotor change induces a non-adaptive proprioception change about one-tenth as large as the oculomotor change. These findings are capable of accounting for the previously unexplained results reported by Wooster in 1923, and also for the current controversy about the role of reafferent stimulation in sensorymotor adaptation. 相似文献
29.
D. J. Murray 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1965,17(1):47-56
An experiment is reported in which the subject read visually presented lists with four different degrees of vocalization; immediately after reading each list he was required to reproduce it either aloud or in writing. Each list consisted of eight consonants and presentation rates were varied between I and 4 letters per sec. For any given series of lists, the subject was asked either to read the letters silently, or to mouth them silently, or to whisper them, or to say them aloud while reading.
At the fastest presentation-rate immediate recall improved monotonically with the degree of vocalization during reading of the lists; at slower rates this generalization held less well, especially for the lower degrees of vocalization. Vocalization was most helpful at the highest presentation-rate.
The overall amount correctly recalled was better for more slowly presented lists and for written as opposed to spoken recall. Analysis of the errors suggested that acoustic confusions were affected by the conditions of presentation; and that serial order intrusions were independent of presentation-or recall-conditions. An apparent variation of transpositions with voicing-and-recall-method failed to reach statistical significance. Theoretical implications of the experiment are discussed, including reference to Broadbent's theory of short-term memory (1958). 相似文献
At the fastest presentation-rate immediate recall improved monotonically with the degree of vocalization during reading of the lists; at slower rates this generalization held less well, especially for the lower degrees of vocalization. Vocalization was most helpful at the highest presentation-rate.
The overall amount correctly recalled was better for more slowly presented lists and for written as opposed to spoken recall. Analysis of the errors suggested that acoustic confusions were affected by the conditions of presentation; and that serial order intrusions were independent of presentation-or recall-conditions. An apparent variation of transpositions with voicing-and-recall-method failed to reach statistical significance. Theoretical implications of the experiment are discussed, including reference to Broadbent's theory of short-term memory (1958). 相似文献
30.