全文获取类型
收费全文 | 27481篇 |
免费 | 507篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
27991篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 275篇 |
2019年 | 291篇 |
2018年 | 338篇 |
2017年 | 378篇 |
2016年 | 425篇 |
2015年 | 328篇 |
2014年 | 367篇 |
2013年 | 1985篇 |
2012年 | 671篇 |
2011年 | 699篇 |
2010年 | 428篇 |
2009年 | 454篇 |
2008年 | 636篇 |
2007年 | 568篇 |
2006年 | 515篇 |
2005年 | 506篇 |
2004年 | 461篇 |
2003年 | 469篇 |
2002年 | 483篇 |
2001年 | 697篇 |
2000年 | 689篇 |
1999年 | 480篇 |
1992年 | 476篇 |
1991年 | 471篇 |
1990年 | 474篇 |
1989年 | 470篇 |
1988年 | 423篇 |
1987年 | 432篇 |
1986年 | 459篇 |
1985年 | 495篇 |
1984年 | 425篇 |
1983年 | 383篇 |
1982年 | 293篇 |
1981年 | 338篇 |
1980年 | 258篇 |
1979年 | 451篇 |
1978年 | 331篇 |
1977年 | 297篇 |
1976年 | 285篇 |
1975年 | 422篇 |
1974年 | 475篇 |
1973年 | 495篇 |
1972年 | 413篇 |
1971年 | 409篇 |
1970年 | 386篇 |
1969年 | 403篇 |
1968年 | 499篇 |
1967年 | 480篇 |
1966年 | 457篇 |
1958年 | 257篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
321.
In this paper, we examine whether color and shape, tied to a single object in space, (1) are identified and selected in series or in parallel, (2) are identified and selected in a dependent, self-terminating manner or in an independent and exhaustive manner, and (3) are conjoined by a feature integration process before or only after an initial stage of separate attribute analyses has finished. We measured response time and the selection negativity (SN) derived from event-related brain potentials when participants responded to a unique conjunction of color and shape in a go/no-go target detection task. The discriminability of the color and the shape of the conjunction was manipulated in three conditions. When color and shape were easy to discriminate, the SNs to color and shape started at the same time. When one attribute was less discriminable, the SN to that attribute started later, but not the SN to the complementary attribute. This suggests that color and shape are identified and selected in parallel. In all three discriminability conditions, the SNs to color and shape were initially independent but later interacted. This suggests that color and shape are initially selected independently and exhaustively, after which their conjunction is analyzed. The SN to local shape features started later than that to the conjunction of color and global shape features, which suggests that feature integration can start before the analyses of the separate attributes have finished. 相似文献
322.
Joan M Sinnott Charles H. Brown Waseem T. Malik Regina A. Kressley 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1997,59(8):1214-1224
Multidimensional scaling (MDS) was used to compare perceptual maps for 10 synthetic English vowels in humans and Old World monkeys (Macaca fuscata andCercopithecus albogularis). Subjects discriminated among the vowels using a repeating background procedure, and reaction times were submitted to an MDS analysis to derive measures of perceived similarity. The dimensions that emerged related to the frequencies of the first(F1), second(F2), and third(F3) formants. Human data indicated a good match to previous MDS studies using rating procedures or confusion matrices: The dominant dimension mapped onto vowelF2, the phonetically most important formant, and the second and third dimensions mapped ontoF1 andF3, respectively.For monkeys, equal weightings occurred forF1 andF2, andF3 was not clearly represented. Monkey sensitivity to the formants appeared to relate to formant amplitudes. If monkeys are giving an accurate representation of the psychoacoustic relations among the formants, then our human results suggest that species-specific mechanisms, reflecting the salience of the phonetic feature of advancement, may contribute to vowel coding in humans. 相似文献
323.
In a previous study of two-choice reactions, pairings of spatial stimuli with bimanual presses made on a keyboard and verbal stimuli with unimanual aimed movements made on a display screen showed higher set-level compatibility than the opposite pairings; element-level compatibility (i.e., mapping) effects were also larger for the conditions with high set-level compatibility than for those whose set-level compatibility was low. In the 4 experiments described here, the relevant factors were isolated, allowing the determinants of those compatibility differences to be evaluated in more detail. Forty-eight students participated in Experiment 1, and 24 each in Experiments 2, 3, and 4. The primary determinant of the set-level compatibility variation was whether the response alternatives involved 1 or 2 effectors, but the differences in element-level compatibility effects were determined primarily by the distinction between responding on the screen as opposed to on the keyboard. Implications for models of stimulus-response compatibility are examined. 相似文献
324.
Samuel F. Sears Jr. James R. Rodrigue Anthony F. Greene Peter Fauerbach Roger M. Mills Jr. 《Journal of religion and health》1997,36(4):345-352
Previous research has established the existence of homogeneous religious coping profiles in cardiac-transplantation candidates labeled as the deferring/collaborators, self-directors, and the eclectic religious copers. However, their prospective impact on outcome has not yet been established. This paper examines potential differences between pre-cardiac transplantation religious coping cluster groups on post-cardiac transplantation quality of life (physical functioning, mental health, and general health). Results indicated that the religious coping profiles of deferring/collaborators and self-directors had significantly better scores on mental health and general health than did the eclectics. Implications for religious-coping research and clinical practice are discussed. 相似文献
325.
The Revd. Professor Leslie J. Francis The Revd. Susan H. Jones 《Pastoral Psychology》1997,45(6):421-428
A sample of 368 committed Christian adults completed the Index of Charismatic Experience together with the short form Revised
Eysenck Personality Questionnaire and the Myers Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI). The data demonstrate that, according to the
MBTI typology, Christians who prefer “thinking” are likely to score higher on the Index of Charismatic Experience than Christians
who prefer “feeling”. According to the Eysenckian dimensional model of personality, the charismatic experience is associated
with stable extraversion. 相似文献
326.
R. Grossarth-Maticek H.J. Eysenck A. Pfeifer P. Schmidt G. Koppel 《Personality and individual differences》1997,23(6):949-960
We report the results of a prospective study of a random sample of 1353 elderly but healthy men and women who were followed up for a period of 10 yr, when mortality and cause of death were ascertained. Overall results have been reported elsewhere for mortality from cancer, coronary heart disease and other causes; here we are concerned with the personality factors predicting breast cancer, cervical cancer, cancer of the corpus uteri specifically, as well as other types of cancer in women. Ten specific hypotheses were tested, using specially constructed questionnaires for the purpose, and for most of these statistically significant support was obtained. The results justify belief that specific types of cancer may be related to specific stress/personality factors. 相似文献
327.
MICHAEL S. FRIEDMANN WILSON H. McDERMUT DAVID A. SOLOMON CHRISTINE E. RYAN GABOR I. KEITNER IVAN W. MILLER 《Family process》1997,36(4):357-367
The primary objective of the present investigation was to examine adaptive functioning in the families of patients with a wide range of psychiatric disorders. Seven dimensions of family functioning, as measured by the Family Assessment Device (FAD), were compared across families of patients with a schizophrenia spectrum disorder (n = 61 ), bipolar disorder (n = 60 ), major depression (n = 111 ), anxiety disorder (n = 15 ), eating disorder (n = 26 ), substance abuse disorder (n = 48 ), and adjustment disorder (n = 46 ). Families in each psychiatric group were also compared to a control group of nonclinical families (N = 353 ). Results indicated that regardless of specific diagnosis, having a family member in an acute phase of a psychiatric illness was a risk factor for poor family functioning compared to the functioning of control families. However, with few exceptions, the type of the patient's psychiatric illness did not predict significant differences in family functioning. Thus, having a family member with a psychiatric illness is a general stressor for families, and family interventions should be considered for most patients who require a psychiatric hospitalization for either the onset of, or an acute exacerbation of, any psychiatric disorder. 相似文献
328.
329.
The American Journal of Psychoanalysis - 相似文献
330.
The authors examined the role of "perceived loomingness" in fear of HIV. Perceived loomingness refers to perceptions of rapid forward movement and instantaneous changes in the distance and danger of a potential threat (Riskind, 1992). One hundred and twenty undergraduates rated vignettes of two public encounters with an HIV-positive stranger. High-HIV fear subjects perceived greater loomingness and danger in these vignettes than did low-HIV fear subjects. Regression analyses that tested for a mediated model confirmed that the perceptions of loomingness may spark threat cognitions (such as the probability and imminence of harm), which, in turn, lead to fear. As predicted by the harm-looming model, loomingness also had some effects on fear that were not mediated by such standard threat cognitions. 相似文献