首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   773篇
  免费   67篇
  2023年   11篇
  2022年   11篇
  2021年   17篇
  2020年   34篇
  2019年   41篇
  2018年   40篇
  2017年   53篇
  2016年   45篇
  2015年   26篇
  2014年   30篇
  2013年   83篇
  2012年   33篇
  2011年   39篇
  2010年   22篇
  2009年   17篇
  2008年   26篇
  2007年   20篇
  2006年   22篇
  2005年   18篇
  2004年   22篇
  2003年   14篇
  2002年   15篇
  2001年   11篇
  2000年   11篇
  1999年   4篇
  1998年   5篇
  1996年   4篇
  1995年   9篇
  1994年   8篇
  1992年   12篇
  1991年   8篇
  1990年   7篇
  1988年   6篇
  1987年   4篇
  1984年   3篇
  1983年   6篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   9篇
  1978年   3篇
  1977年   7篇
  1976年   3篇
  1975年   4篇
  1973年   5篇
  1972年   5篇
  1971年   8篇
  1970年   3篇
  1969年   8篇
  1968年   6篇
  1967年   7篇
  1966年   3篇
排序方式: 共有840条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
181.
The representation of uniform motion in vision   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
M T Swanston  N J Wade  R H Day 《Perception》1987,16(2):143-159
For veridical detection of object motion any moving detecting system must allocate motion appropriately between itself and objects in space. A model for such allocation is developed for simplified situations (points of light in uniform motion in a frontoparallel plane). It is proposed that motion of objects is registered and represented successively at four levels within frames of reference that are defined by the detectors themselves or by their movements. The four levels are referred to as retinocentric, orbitocentric, egocentric, and geocentric. Thus the retinocentric signal is combined with that for eye rotation to give an orbitocentric signal, and the left and right orbitocentric signals are combined to give an egocentric representation. Up to the egocentric level, motion representation is angular rather than three-dimensional. The egocentric signal is combined with signals for head and body movement and for egocentric distance to give a geocentric representation. It is argued that although motion perception is always geocentric, relevant registrations also occur at the three earlier levels. The model is applied to various veridical and nonveridical motion phenomena.  相似文献   
182.
183.
In a factorial design, athletic involvement (no varsity sport, varsity noncontact sport, varsity contact sport) was varied with induced disposition (unprovoked, provoked). Under conditions of no provocation, no significant differences in aggressiveness were observed between nonathletes and athletes, nor between noncontact- and contact-sport athletes. In contrast, under conditions of provocation, nonathletes displayed more aggressiveness than athletes. Non-contact-sport athletes behaved significantly less aggressively than both nonathletes and contact-sport athletes. Contact-sport athletes failed to behave significantly less aggressively than nonathletes, however. The findings were explained as the result of an acquired superior ability in athletes to cope with provocation under competitive circumstances, which is partially counteracted in contact-sport athletes by their aggressiveness habits and disinhibition training.  相似文献   
184.
185.
186.
187.
A series of random-shaped polygons varying in number of sides in approximately even logarithmic steps from four to 160 sides was generated. Ss were required to compare all possible pairs of figures on one of three scales-subjective complexity, pleasingness and interestingness. Subjective evaluations of complexity continued to increase with informational content. Pleasingness evaluations described a bimodal function, peafeing at the 6-sided and 28-sided levels then falling rapidly with increased complexity. Interestingness efaluations rosé to a peafe at the 28-sided figure and remained high throughout the rest of the series.  相似文献   
188.
189.
190.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号