全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1474篇 |
免费 | 24篇 |
专业分类
1498篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 11篇 |
2023年 | 25篇 |
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 28篇 |
2020年 | 35篇 |
2019年 | 23篇 |
2018年 | 53篇 |
2017年 | 57篇 |
2016年 | 57篇 |
2015年 | 44篇 |
2014年 | 57篇 |
2013年 | 279篇 |
2012年 | 111篇 |
2011年 | 87篇 |
2010年 | 52篇 |
2009年 | 44篇 |
2008年 | 90篇 |
2007年 | 89篇 |
2006年 | 66篇 |
2005年 | 38篇 |
2004年 | 23篇 |
2003年 | 26篇 |
2002年 | 29篇 |
2001年 | 10篇 |
2000年 | 22篇 |
1999年 | 14篇 |
1998年 | 10篇 |
1997年 | 8篇 |
1996年 | 10篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 13篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 4篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1970年 | 2篇 |
1969年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
1946年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有1498条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
André Haynal Professor 《International Forum of Psychoanalysis》2013,22(1-2):14-19
In an historical context focused on a close examination of the complex relationship between Freud and Ferenczi, the author shows Ferenczi's contribution to the evolution of psychoanalysis. He describes how his ideas and his therapeutic sensitivity anticipated modern clinical thought (for example, Winnicott and Bion), especially the understanding of borderline and narcissistic pathology. The paper considers the following topics: transference and countertransference; early affectivity; the different psychic trauma; phenomena connected with dissociation; the healing factor of the analysis. 相似文献
52.
Robert D. Pritchard Satoris S. Youngcourt Joel R. Philo David McMonagle José H. David 《人类行为》2013,26(1):61-83
To provide the most value to an organization, individuals must focus efforts on those outputs that have maximum value to the organization. However, the realities of the workplace are that information about performance and how individuals can add value to an organization is complex; that is, determining which activities to focus on is a fairly complex task because such information is nonlinear, whereby the importance of a certain activity may change depending on the level of performance at any given time. This article empirically examines whether providing complex, nonlinear feedback that reflects the complexities of the real world will result in a change in work outputs that reflect these nonlinearities. This article assesses whether employees can and do use complex feedback that reflects the complexity of the real world in the intended ways. In addition, this article examines whether individuals use feedback that includes priority information beyond feedback that reports on simple relative importance. Data were analyzed from 9 work units for time periods averaging a little over a year. Results indicate that, as predicted, when individuals have access to both complex, nonlinear priority information and simple, linear relative importance information, individuals can and do use the complex data in the intended way. 相似文献
53.
Jesús Varela Mallou José Manuel Sabucedo Cameselle Constantino Arce Fernández 《Estudios de Psicología》2013,34(43-44):207-224
ResumenEl presente artículo tiene corno principal objetivo ser una alternativa a las tradicionales clasificaciones teóricas de la agresión e, incluso, a los estudios taxonómicos empíricos más recientes. Usar estímulos no representativos, ofrecer indicios sobre las dimensiones de juicios preseleccionados, etc. son algunos de los inconvenientes que no permiten considerar a estas dimensiones perceptuales de la agresión como fiables. En este trabajo se pretenden superar estas y otras críticas con el fin de proporcionar las verdaderas dimensiones que subyacen a la percepción social de los episodios agresivos cotidianos. El Escalamiento Multidimensional (Modelo IÑDSCAL) fue seleccionado como el análisis más apropiado para proponer una taxonomía empírica adecuada y comprobar hasta qué punto una misma taxonomía es compartida por diferentes subculturas (Universitaria y Reclusa). El análisis de los datos nos permitió concluir acerca de la existencia de un dominio psicológico en 4 dimensiones para percibir un conjunto de estímulos agresivos, de la existencia de ciertas diferencias entre ambas submuestras y, por último, de la homogeneidad existente dentro de una misma muestra, independientemente de algunas variables de personalidad previamente seleccionadas. 相似文献
54.
Estrella Romero Jorge Sobral M. Angeles Luengo José A. Marzoa 《The Journal of genetic psychology》2013,174(1):20-40
A number of studies have indicated that delinquent adolescents have characteristic value systems. However, most of these studies have adopted a “known-groups” design—that is, they have compared institutionalized delinquents and non-institutionalized adolescents, assuming those adolescents to be non-delinquent. Designs of this type do not distinguish effectively between the statistical effects of delinquency and of institutionalization. In this study, the authors investigated relationships between values and self-reported antisocial behavior in three adolescent groups: 435 school-attending boys, 529 school-attending girls, and 95 delinquent boys in juvenile rehabilitation centers or prisons. The results indicate that antisocial behavior is associated with hedonistic values and a lack of interest in conventional values and social values. These relationships cannot be explained by the institutionalization effects. 相似文献
55.
The aim of this study was to evaluate a procedure to reveal the emergence of full naming in typically developing children. In Experiment 1, five 6-year-old children (a) learned tacts of pictures and the emergence of the selection of these pictures upon hearing their names was tested; and (b) the selection of other pictures was taught and the emergence of the tacts was tested. All children showed the emergence of picture selection and 3 children showed the emergence of the tacts. In Experiment 2a, the children's correct repetition of the names of 3 pictures was verified first, then the experimenter presented the pictures with their names without requiring any behavior other than attending. Finally, the emergence of picture selection and the tacts was tested. Experiment 2b replicated Experiment 2a with words that were easier to pronounce. Picture selection and the tacts emerged; this performance documented full naming for the first time with typically developing children. These procedures can be used to induce naming capacity in children who lack it; hence, they can be applied to teach children with learning delays. 相似文献
56.
57.
Serniclaes W Van Heghe S Mousty P Carré R Sprenger-Charolles L 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2004,87(4):336-361
Perceptual discrimination between speech sounds belonging to different phoneme categories is better than that between sounds falling within the same category. This property, known as "categorical perception," is weaker in children affected by dyslexia. Categorical perception develops from the predispositions of newborns for discriminating all potential phoneme categories in the world's languages. Predispositions that are not relevant for phoneme perception in the ambient language are usually deactivated during early childhood. However, the current study shows that dyslexic children maintain a higher sensitivity to phonemic distinctions irrelevant in their linguistic environment. This suggests that dyslexic children use an allophonic mode of speech perception that, although without straightforward consequences for oral communication, has obvious implications for the acquisition of alphabetic writing. Allophonic perception specifically affects the mapping between graphemes and phonemes, contrary to other manifestations of dyslexia, and may be a core deficit. 相似文献
58.
In this article, normative data on the familiarity and difficulty of 196 single-solution Spanish word fragments are presented.
The database includes the following indices: difficulty, familiarity, frequency, number of meanings, number of letters given
in the fragment, first and/or last letters given, and ratio of letters to blanks. A factor analysis was performed on difficulty,
and two factors were obtained. Frequency, familiarity, and number of meanings loaded highly on the first factor, which we
consider to measure lexical processes, whereas number of letters in the fragment, first and/or last letters given, and ratio
of letters to blanks loaded highly on the second factor, which we judge to be determined by perceptual information. Regression
analyses using factor scores as predictors showed that both factors accounted for a significant part of the completion probability
scores. The full set of these norms may be downloaded from the Psychonomic Society Web archive atwww.psychonomic.org/archive/. 相似文献
59.
Hallé PA Dominguez A Cuetos F Segui J 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2008,34(1):177-192
In a series of 4 experiments, the authors show that phonological repair mechanisms, known to operate in the auditory modality, are directly translated in the visual modality. This holds with the provision that printed stimuli are presented for a very brief duration and that the effect of phonological repair is tested after a delay of some 100 ms has elapsed after that presentation. The case of phonological repair chosen to exemplify the parallelism between print and speech is the prosthesis of /e/ in utterances beginning with /s/ followed by a consonant in Spanish. Native speakers of Spanish hear a prothetic /e/ in auditorily presented pseudowords such as special (/speojal/, derived from "especial") as well as stuto (/stuto/, derived from "astuto"). It is shown here that they also hear that same vowel /e/ when presented with the printed pseudowords "special" and "stuto." This finding of a phonological repair effect in print has implications for the issue of phonological activation from print, as well as for the prelexical locus and mandatory nature of phonological repair mechanisms in general. 相似文献
60.
External validation of the computerized, group administrable adaptation of the “operation span task”
One of the most widely used tasks for measuring working memory capacity is the operation span task (OSPAN; Turner & Engle, 1989). This task has almost always been applied individually, and stimuli presentation is controlled by the experimenter. Recently, De Neys, d'Ydewalle, Schaeken, and Vos (2002) improved the administration procedure by designing an automated, group-administrable version of the task (GOSPAN). They found GOSPAN to be reliable, and they also provided evidence on its validity (a significant positive correlation between GOSPAN and OSPAN scores). However, an external test of GOSPAN validity is still lacking. In this work, we present such a validation for the automated version, when the task is administered both individually (Experiment 1) and to groups (Experiment 2). There are abundant previous data on the relation between working memory capacity and reading comprehension. In this work, this relation is studied using an automated OSPAN version to measure working memory capacity. Given that our results are similar to those found using the original OSPAN, our data support the external validity of the automated version of the task. We also tested the reliability of the task and found high internal consistency in both experiments. 相似文献