全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1542篇 |
免费 | 9篇 |
专业分类
1551篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 17篇 |
2020年 | 24篇 |
2018年 | 29篇 |
2017年 | 22篇 |
2016年 | 31篇 |
2015年 | 20篇 |
2014年 | 26篇 |
2013年 | 165篇 |
2012年 | 27篇 |
2011年 | 40篇 |
2010年 | 32篇 |
2009年 | 37篇 |
2008年 | 61篇 |
2007年 | 66篇 |
2006年 | 46篇 |
2005年 | 38篇 |
2004年 | 54篇 |
2003年 | 60篇 |
2002年 | 47篇 |
2001年 | 18篇 |
2000年 | 19篇 |
1999年 | 27篇 |
1998年 | 20篇 |
1997年 | 21篇 |
1996年 | 23篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 19篇 |
1993年 | 20篇 |
1992年 | 25篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 23篇 |
1988年 | 17篇 |
1987年 | 16篇 |
1986年 | 16篇 |
1985年 | 15篇 |
1984年 | 20篇 |
1983年 | 18篇 |
1982年 | 25篇 |
1981年 | 19篇 |
1980年 | 17篇 |
1979年 | 27篇 |
1978年 | 22篇 |
1977年 | 27篇 |
1976年 | 23篇 |
1975年 | 20篇 |
1974年 | 15篇 |
1973年 | 21篇 |
1970年 | 12篇 |
1968年 | 11篇 |
1967年 | 13篇 |
排序方式: 共有1551条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
971.
Many objects typically occur in particular locations, and object words encode these spatial associations. We tested whether such object words (e.g., head, foot) orient attention toward the location where the denoted object typically occurs (i.e., up, down). Because object words elicit perceptual simulations of the denoted objects (i.e., the representations acquired during actual perception are reactivated), we predicted that an object word would interfere with identification of an unrelated visual target subsequently presented in the object's typical location. Consistent with this prediction, three experiments demonstrated that words denoting objects that typically occur high in the visual field hindered identification of targets appearing at the top of the display, whereas words denoting low objects hindered target identification at the bottom of the display. Thus, object words oriented attention to and activated perceptual simulations in the objects' typical locations. These results shed new light on how language affects perception. 相似文献
972.
Lollipop Test scores were acquired for 1,402 preschoolers in southwestern Indiana. Means and standard deviations for both boys and girls by 6-mo. intervals from the ages of 42 through 65 months showed scores increased significantly across ages, with girls scoring significantly higher than boys in all age groups. Current mean scores are similar to those published in 1988 for boys and slightly higher for girls. 相似文献
973.
974.
975.
In this research, anticipatory perception of an approaching vehicle was examined. By varying the availability of visual and acoustic media, conditions occurred in which the input to the modalities was repetitively interrupted while the presentation of the approach event continued. In these conditions, the audio and the visual signals were presented either in short and concurrent segments at regular intervals or in alternation (interleaved) at regular intervals. It was found that interrupting the signal within a single modality did not affect performance if the approach information was available in the alternate modality. These results are consistent with a modally flexible detection mechanism for the perception of approaching objects. This modal flexibility may provide some evidence that information is detected using a modality-neutral strategy. 相似文献
976.
977.
Lawrence E. Marks 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1980,27(6):489-498
A series of six experiments used the method of magnitude estimation to assess how the two ears sum the loudness of stimuli with various spectra. The results showed that the binaural system sums loudnesses by at least two distinct sets of rules, one applicable to narrow-band stimuli (complete loudness summation), another to wide-band noises (partial summation, dependent on level). The main findings were: (1) Narrow-band noise (Vi-octave bands at 1,000 Hz) showed complete binaural loudness summation, like that previously reported for pure tones (Marks, 1978a). At all but low SPL, a monaural stimulus must be 10 dB greater than a binaural stimulus to be equally loud; a stimulus ratio of 10 dB corresponds to a loudness ratio of 2:1 on Stevens’ sone scale. (2) Wide-band noise (300-4,800 Hz) showed only partial summation, the subadditivity being confined largely to levels below about 60 dB SPL. This result obtained both with bands of white noise (flat spectrum) and pink noise (—3 dB/ octave). (3) Binaural summation of two-tone complexes depended slightly on frequency spacing. Narrow spacing (860 and 1,160 Hz) gave summation equal to about 10 dB, like that of narrowband noises and single tones, whereas wider spacing (675 and 1,475 Hz) gave less summation, equal to about 9 dB, and more like wide-band noise; however, a very wide spacing (300 and 4,800 Hz) gave summation like that of narrow-band noises and single pure tones. 相似文献
978.
Lawrence J. Hubert 《Journal of mathematical psychology》1980,21(3):247-264
A data analysis strategy is discussed for evaluating the degree to which a subset D of a larger object set S satisfies a particular algebraic property. Based on a set measure f(D) and a proximity function on S × S, two separate evaluation tasks, referred to as confirmatory and exploratory, are considered. In a confirmatory task the subset D is identified a priori and f(D) is compared against the distribution of f(·) over all subsets containing the same number of objects. The exploratory task, on the other hand, treats f(·) as an objective function to be optimized over all subsets of a given size. Examples of these two notions include the assessment of symmetry, cluster compactness, and the extent to which D satisfies the error-free conditions for a hierarchical model or a unidimensional scale. 相似文献
979.
980.
Lawrence W. Jaffe 《Psychological Perspectives》2013,56(2):152-156
On polarity: Wilfred sheed's max jamisen eudora warn's losing battles 相似文献