全文获取类型
收费全文 | 6318篇 |
免费 | 135篇 |
专业分类
6453篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 72篇 |
2019年 | 93篇 |
2018年 | 115篇 |
2017年 | 118篇 |
2016年 | 120篇 |
2015年 | 103篇 |
2014年 | 111篇 |
2013年 | 539篇 |
2012年 | 212篇 |
2011年 | 227篇 |
2010年 | 142篇 |
2009年 | 130篇 |
2008年 | 156篇 |
2007年 | 150篇 |
2006年 | 146篇 |
2005年 | 114篇 |
2004年 | 115篇 |
2003年 | 112篇 |
2002年 | 122篇 |
2001年 | 202篇 |
2000年 | 193篇 |
1999年 | 155篇 |
1998年 | 56篇 |
1993年 | 54篇 |
1992年 | 112篇 |
1991年 | 113篇 |
1990年 | 107篇 |
1989年 | 94篇 |
1988年 | 89篇 |
1987年 | 93篇 |
1986年 | 99篇 |
1985年 | 103篇 |
1984年 | 91篇 |
1983年 | 72篇 |
1982年 | 52篇 |
1981年 | 56篇 |
1980年 | 56篇 |
1979年 | 109篇 |
1978年 | 56篇 |
1977年 | 52篇 |
1976年 | 62篇 |
1975年 | 76篇 |
1974年 | 104篇 |
1973年 | 91篇 |
1972年 | 78篇 |
1971年 | 78篇 |
1970年 | 77篇 |
1969年 | 83篇 |
1968年 | 75篇 |
1967年 | 88篇 |
排序方式: 共有6453条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
921.
Previous research has demonstrated electroencephalogram (EEG) changes in response to low-odor concentrations, resulting in near-chance detection. Such findings have been taken as evidence for olfaction without awareness. We replicated and extended previous work by examining EEG responses to water-water control, 0.0001, 0. 001, 0.01, and 1 ppm isoamyl acetate (IAA) in water paired with water only. Detection was above chance (>50%) for.001 and above, and alpha decreased only to those concentrations, suggesting that EEG changes corresponded to IAA awareness. However, when correct trial EEGs were compared to incorrect trial EEGs during.001 ppm, right posterior/central alpha decreased during incorrect trials and alpha decreased more globally (including frontal sites) during correct trials. These data may not reflect awareness or unawareness per se. Instead, results are discussed regarding activation of perceptual systems in the posterior region during incorrect trials and the activation of frontal action systems during a subset of correct trials. 相似文献
922.
This article is based on accumulated clinical experience in Israel with families that emigrated from the former Soviet Union. It describes a culturally sensitive systemic intervention with two such families: a single-parent family, and a family that exhibited physical violence. Relevant cultural characteristics of family patterns and parent-child relationships in Jewish-Soviet families are reviewed. It is demonstrated how a cross-cultural perspective may affect the interpretation of presented problems and result in a less pathological perspective. It is further illustrated how universal intervention techniques combined with culturally sensitive approaches may produce positive effects in therapy. 相似文献
923.
Observers viewed monocular animations of rotating dihedral angles and were required to indicate their perceived structures by adjusting the magnitude and orientation of a stereoscopic dihedral angle. The motion displays were created by directly manipulating various aspects of the image velocity field, including the mean translation, the horizontal and vertical velocity gradients, and the manner in which these gradients changed over time. The adjusted orientation of each planar facet was decomposed into components of slant and tilt. Although the tilt component was estimated with a high degree of accuracy, the judgments of slant exhibited large systematic errors. The magnitude of perceived slant was determined primarily by the magnitude of the velocity gradient scaled by its direction. The results also indicate that higher order temporal derivatives of the moving elements had little effect on observers' judgments. 相似文献
924.
Daniel V. Eastmond 《New Directions for Adult & Continuing Education》1998,1998(78):33-41
Adult learners respond differently to different types of technology. This chapter presents a typology of Internet-based instruction that relates levels of technology to characteristics of adult learning. 相似文献
925.
Maree Hunt Wendy V. Parr Peter Smith 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology》1999,52(3):203-233
The present study employed a behavioural detection approach to investigate the combined effects of sample duration and sample presentation frequency on delayed matching accuracy of pigeons. Experiment 1 showed that when both samples were exposed at each of the two possible durations within a two-alternative, delayed matching session, discriminability was higher to the longer duration sample than to the short-duration sample, as found when sample duration is varied between sessions. Experiment 2's asymmetrical procedure increased bias toward the more frequent of the two samples but had no influence on discriminability. Initial discriminability was higher to samples exposed for longer durations, irrespective of stimulus presentation frequency. The results suggest qualitatively different effects of the two sources of stimulus control under consideration: Sample duration (a local or within-trial manipulation) exerted its effect on discrimination of the stimuli, whereas sample presentation frequence (a global factor) exerted its major effect on response bias. An interpretation of the data in terms of Blough's (1996) analysis of errors in matching tasks suggests that the amount of behaviour under control of the sample stimulus markedly changed with different sample durations. The analysis also showed that the biasing manipulation exerted most of its effect on the portion of behaviour outside of control by the critical stimulus. We argue that theoretical accounts of delayed matching performance need to consider both local and global factors as determinants of matching accuracy. 相似文献
926.
It has been shown that visual awareness in the blind hemifield of hemianopic cats that have undergone unilateral ablations of visual cortex can be restored by sectioning the commissure of the superior colliculus or by destroying a portion of the substantia nigra contralateral to the cortical lesion (the Sprague effect). We propose that the visual awareness that is recovered is due to synchronized oscillatory activities in the superior colliculus ipsilateral to the cortical lesion. These oscillatory activities are normally partially suppressed by the inhibitory, GABAergic contralateral nigrotectal projection, and the destruction of the substantia nigra, or the sectioning of the collicular commissure, disinhibits the collicular neurons, causing an increase in the extent of oscillatory activity and/or synchronization between activities at different sites. This increase in the oscillatory and synchronized character is sufficient for the activities to give rise to visual awareness. We argue that in rodents and lower vertebrates, normal visual awareness is partly due to synchronized oscillatory activities in the optic tectum and partly due to similar activities in visual cortex. It is only in carnivores and primates that visual awareness is wholly due to cortical activities. Based on von Baerian recapitulation theory, we propose that, even in humans, there is a period in early infancy when visual awareness is partially due to activities in the superior colliculus, but that this awareness gradually disappears as the nigrotectal projection matures. 相似文献
927.
V. Anne Tubb James M. Wood Harmon M. Hosch 《Journal of applied social psychology》1999,29(6):1111-1127
928.
929.
In three experiments, a rat's lever presses could postpone timeouts from food pellets delivered on response-independent schedules. In Experiment 1, the pellets were delivered at variable-time (VT) rates ranging from VT 0.5 to VT 8 min. Experiment 2 replicated the VT 1 min and VT 8 min conditions of Experiment 1 with new subjects. Finally, subjects in Experiment 3 could postpone timeouts from delivery of pellets that differed in quality rather than quantity (unsweetened versus sweetened pellets). In general, response rates and success in avoiding increased as a function of the rate and quality of the pellets. Also, performance efficiency increased as the experiments progressed, that is, the avoidance response occurred later and later in the response-timeout interval. The results support the conclusion that timeout from reinforcement has functional properties similar to those of more commonly studied aversive stimuli (e.g., shock). 相似文献
930.
Kimberly D. Bess Isaac Prilleltensky Douglas D. Perkins Leslie V. Collins 《American journal of community psychology》2009,43(1-2):134-148
Community psychologists have long worked with community-based human service organizations to build participatory processes. These efforts largely aim at building participatory practices within the current individual-wellness paradigm of human services. To address collective wellness, human service organizations need to challenge their current paradigm, attend to the social justice needs of community, and engage community participation in a new way, and in doing so become more openly political. We use qualitative interviews, focus groups, organizational documents, and participant observation to present a comparative case study of two organizations involved in such a process through an action research project aimed at transforming the organizations’ managerial and practice paradigm from one based on first-order, ameliorative change to one that promotes second-order, transformative change via strength-based approaches, primary prevention, empowerment and participation, and focuses on changing community conditions. Four participatory tensions or dialectics are discussed: passive versus active participation, partners versus clients, surplus powerlessness versus collective efficacy, and reflection/learning versus action/doing. 相似文献