首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   77611篇
  免费   3034篇
  国内免费   40篇
  2020年   831篇
  2019年   1057篇
  2018年   1477篇
  2017年   1550篇
  2016年   1587篇
  2015年   1085篇
  2014年   1321篇
  2013年   6561篇
  2012年   2314篇
  2011年   2582篇
  2010年   1572篇
  2009年   1582篇
  2008年   2354篇
  2007年   2272篇
  2006年   2021篇
  2005年   1804篇
  2004年   1715篇
  2003年   1655篇
  2002年   1652篇
  2001年   2598篇
  2000年   2511篇
  1999年   1865篇
  1998年   959篇
  1997年   818篇
  1996年   741篇
  1995年   754篇
  1994年   753篇
  1993年   750篇
  1992年   1562篇
  1991年   1495篇
  1990年   1435篇
  1989年   1321篇
  1988年   1262篇
  1987年   1266篇
  1986年   1227篇
  1985年   1349篇
  1984年   1054篇
  1983年   873篇
  1979年   1046篇
  1978年   786篇
  1976年   706篇
  1975年   928篇
  1974年   971篇
  1973年   1005篇
  1972年   865篇
  1971年   744篇
  1969年   717篇
  1968年   866篇
  1967年   817篇
  1966年   802篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 78 毫秒
901.
Previous researchers using between-subjects comparisons have found eyewitness confidence and accuracy to be only negligibly correlated. In this study, we examined the predictive power of confidence in within-subject terms. Ninety-six subjects answered, and made confidence ratings for, a series of questions about a crime they witnessed. The average between-subjects and within-subject accuracy-confidence correlations were comparably low: r = .14 (p less than .001) and r = .17 (p less than .001), respectively. Confidence is neither a useful predictor of the accuracy of a particular witness nor of the accuracy of particular statements made by the same witness. Another possible predictor of accuracy, response latency, correlated only negligibly with accuracy (r = -.09 within subjects), but more strongly with confidence (r = -.27 within subjects). This pattern was obtained for both between-subjects and within-subject comparisons. The theoretical and practical implications of these results are discussed.  相似文献   
902.
Observing responses of college students and children of different ages (4 to 5, 9 to 10, and 13 to 14 years) were studied using two-component schedules of reinforcement. Different frequencies or numbers of points were given in the components and points were backed by either money or instructions about getting points (e.g., “points are for the experimenter's information only”). Stimuli that were either correlated or uncorrelated with the components were contingent on presses on either of two concurrently available levers (observing responses). In each of three experiments, preferences for stimuli were consistent with the conditioned-reinforcement hypothesis of observing but inconsistent with the uncertainty-reduction hypothesis. Stimulus preferences were established, eliminated, or reversed by instructions about the significance of getting points not backed with money. Instructions to in effect ignore points with no monetary value eliminated stimulus preference provided that money was not associated with points in other conditions for the same subjects. Stimulus preferences were otherwise independent of monetary point value. Instructions that described reinforcement contingencies promoted more rapid adjustment to the contingencies than occurred without them, and preferences were less variable across sessions within subjects and were on the average more extreme. Although some results involving absolute rates of observing depended upon the age of subjects, stimulus preferences did not. The data suggest that the conditioned-reinforcement hypothesis of observing has considerable generality for humans in choice procedures in the laboratory. That monetary value of points did not affect preferences in a graded fashion is consistent with versions of the conditioned-reinforcement hypothesis which emphasize reduction in delay to reinforcement over other reinforcement parameters.  相似文献   
903.
904.
905.
Two studies using a computer-simulated, strategy-formulation game and business students were conducted using simultaneous verbal protocols. It was found that a number of information-processing and information-evaluation thought processes were significantly related to game performance. Consistent with what is known about individual decision making, the present results suggest that, for the task used in this study, individuals who engage in causal analysis perform better than those who do not. But those who focus on negative emotions, blindly repeat previously successful decisions, and engage in illogical through processes perform more poorly than those who do not. Causal replication using real managers within organizational settings, however, is needed before firm conclusions can be drawn from this research.  相似文献   
906.
907.
908.
909.
910.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号