首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   78464篇
  免费   3070篇
  国内免费   40篇
  2020年   845篇
  2019年   1074篇
  2018年   1500篇
  2017年   1567篇
  2016年   1610篇
  2015年   1103篇
  2014年   1340篇
  2013年   6661篇
  2012年   2339篇
  2011年   2621篇
  2010年   1590篇
  2009年   1603篇
  2008年   2381篇
  2007年   2312篇
  2006年   2051篇
  2005年   1833篇
  2004年   1741篇
  2003年   1677篇
  2002年   1671篇
  2001年   2616篇
  2000年   2527篇
  1999年   1887篇
  1998年   977篇
  1997年   824篇
  1996年   754篇
  1995年   763篇
  1994年   762篇
  1993年   756篇
  1992年   1570篇
  1991年   1502篇
  1990年   1441篇
  1989年   1330篇
  1988年   1271篇
  1987年   1274篇
  1986年   1237篇
  1985年   1358篇
  1984年   1065篇
  1983年   880篇
  1979年   1058篇
  1978年   793篇
  1976年   716篇
  1975年   935篇
  1974年   979篇
  1973年   1020篇
  1972年   872篇
  1971年   749篇
  1969年   724篇
  1968年   871篇
  1967年   822篇
  1966年   807篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
961.
Food-deprived Drosophila melanogaster extend their proboscises following sucrose stimulation of the front tarsi (the proboscis extension reflex). Médioni and Vaysse (1975) reported that the inhibition of this response can be conditioned over trials if such proboscis extensions are punished by applying an aversive stimulus to the foreleg tarsi. In this study, Médioni and Vaysse's basic observations of conditioning were replicated, with a different strain of flies and a modified conditioning apparatus.  相似文献   
962.
Two adult male chimpanzees reached through a hole in the wall of their home cage and, by tracking the images of their hands and of an otherwise hidden target object in a mirror or closed-circuit television picture, moved their hands in whichever direction was necessary to make contact with the target object. They discriminated between live video images and tapes and performed effectively when the target objects were presented in novel locations and when the video picture was presented at random in different orientations. There was thus no consistent relation between the location of images on the monitor and the location of their real-world counterparts. Comparable performances in monkeys and nonprimates seem unlikely.  相似文献   
963.
Bornstein’s hypothesis that there is a loss of color discrimination in tropical areas was not supported in earlier research in the Cook Islands. Using a more sensitive color vision test (the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-Hue Test), a loss of color discrimination in the red-purple and blue-green regions of the spectrum was found. The lack of sex differences in this color discrimination loss supports a physiological rather than a genetic explanation of the observed decrement.  相似文献   
964.
965.
Two experiments investigated whether the initial organization of ambiguous pictures presented supraliminally could be influenced by the presentation of semantically related pictures that had been pattern masked to prevent conscious representation. Twenty-two pictures were presented tachistoscopically in a counterbalanced sequence, each alternating with the supraliminal presentation of its ambiguous counterpart. The results supported the hypothesis that pictures precluded from consciousness by pattern masking could nevertheless influence the interpretation of ambiguous pictures. Although the possibility of structural priming could not be completely ruled out in these experiments, there is evidence to suggest that semantic priming is a component. The research potential of semantic priming is also discussed.  相似文献   
966.
967.
968.
Five- and 7-month-old infants were tested for sensitivity to the depth cue of shading. Infants were presented with two displays: a surface in which a convexity and a concavity were molded and a photograph in which shading specified a convexity and a concavity. Each display was presented under both monocular and binocular viewing conditions. Reaching was observed as the dependent measure. Infants in both age groups reached preferentially for the actual convexity in both the monocular and binocular viewing conditions. In the monocular photograph condition, the 7-month-olds reached preferentially for the apparent convexity specified by shading, indicating that they perceived it to be an actual convexity. These infants showed no significant reaching preference in the binocular photograph condition. This finding rules out interpretations of the infants’ reaching not based on perceived depth. The results therefore suggest that the 7-month-olds perceived depth from shading. The 5-month-olds showed no significant reaching preferences when viewing the photograph; thus, they showed no evidence of depth perception from shading. These findings are consistent with the results of a number of studies that have investigated infants’ sensitivity to pictorial depth cues. Together, these studies suggest that the ability to perceive depth from pictorial cues may first develop between 5 and 7 months of age.  相似文献   
969.
Forty-eight American children, 12 popular and 12 rejected children from Grades 3 and 6, were paired with same-sex friends and nonfriends on an interactive task. Children were videotaped and their interactions judged for the amount and type of attention each child in the dyad gave to each other and the conversation. Main dependent measures included mutual engagements, acknowledgement of the partner, getting the attention of the partner, attention to the conversation, and social impact of the utterances. Both rejected and popular children attended to the conversation and the partner, but rejected children appeared to overattend in several ways. The interactions of rejected children and their matches involved more mutual engagements, conversational initiators, facilitators, terminators, and nonverbal attention-getting devices. Sex and developmental effects were also found.  相似文献   
970.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号