全文获取类型
收费全文 | 81013篇 |
免费 | 1425篇 |
国内免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
82444篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 1009篇 |
2019年 | 1279篇 |
2018年 | 1488篇 |
2017年 | 1565篇 |
2016年 | 1793篇 |
2015年 | 1303篇 |
2014年 | 1354篇 |
2013年 | 6601篇 |
2012年 | 2339篇 |
2011年 | 2619篇 |
2010年 | 1609篇 |
2009年 | 1600篇 |
2008年 | 2373篇 |
2007年 | 2287篇 |
2006年 | 2029篇 |
2005年 | 1813篇 |
2004年 | 1728篇 |
2003年 | 1668篇 |
2002年 | 1659篇 |
2001年 | 2609篇 |
2000年 | 2522篇 |
1999年 | 1873篇 |
1998年 | 977篇 |
1997年 | 830篇 |
1996年 | 751篇 |
1995年 | 762篇 |
1994年 | 768篇 |
1993年 | 758篇 |
1992年 | 1567篇 |
1991年 | 1500篇 |
1990年 | 1438篇 |
1989年 | 1327篇 |
1988年 | 1269篇 |
1987年 | 1269篇 |
1986年 | 1228篇 |
1985年 | 1354篇 |
1984年 | 1058篇 |
1983年 | 882篇 |
1979年 | 1054篇 |
1978年 | 794篇 |
1976年 | 711篇 |
1975年 | 934篇 |
1974年 | 982篇 |
1973年 | 1008篇 |
1972年 | 869篇 |
1971年 | 748篇 |
1969年 | 720篇 |
1968年 | 869篇 |
1967年 | 822篇 |
1966年 | 806篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
291.
292.
The effects of altering light pattern sequences on driver compliance at a busy, urban intersection were explored. The baseline light timing sequences resulted in only 46.8% of drivers stopping at the yellow or red lights. Using an A-B-C design, we altered light pattern sequences that increased the probability of drivers stopping at the signals to 88.8% and 98.8%. These findings indicate that traffic light contingencies have potent effects in influencing driver behaviors at busy intersections. Following completion of the study, the traffic engineer approved the permanence of the light timing pattern that increased traffic rule compliance. Accident data collected before and after the light timing changes indicated a reduction in automobile accidents. 相似文献
293.
294.
Gary M. Pace Martin T. Ivancic Glynnis L. Edwards Brian A. Iwata Terry J. Page 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1985,18(3):249-255
We evaluated a procedure for identifying potential reinforcers with profoundly retarded individuals. In Experiment 1, six persons were repeatedly exposed to 16 stimuli, and approach behaviors to each stimulus were used to identify preferred and nonpreferred stimuli. In Experiment 2, we examined the reinforcing properties of preferred and nonpreferred stimuli by delivering them contingently on the occurrence of arbitrarily selected responses. Results revealed that the preferred stimulus conditions typically produced higher rates of responding than did either the baseline or the nonpreferred stimulus conditions, suggesting that the procedure can be used to assess reinforcer value for individuals with limited behavioral repertoires. 相似文献
295.
296.
Gloria S. Waters Margaret Bruck Mark Seidenberg 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1985,39(3):511-530
The use of spelling-sound information in both reading and spelling was evaluated by having children read and spell nonwords and five types of words that differed in terms of their regularity for reading and spelling. The subjects were grade 3 children who had been psychometrically defined as good readers and good spellers (“good”), good readers and poor spellers (“mixed”), or poor readers and poor spellers (“poor”). Results indicated that all children attempted to use spelling-sound correspondences in both reading and spelling, although children in both the mixed and the poor groups had weaker knowledge of these correspondences and were less systematic in their use of them. Furthermore, even though the children in the mixed group had been matched with children in the good group on reading comprehension, the number and type of errors made by the mixed subjects on both the reading and spelling tasks were more similar to those of the poor subjects than to those of the good subjects. 相似文献
297.
S.Stavros Valenti 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1985,40(3):406-419
Data relating novelty preference to age for normal children are inconsistent, although a current theory predicts a developmental shift from novelty to familiarity preference in selective learning (D. Zeaman, 1976, in T. J. Tighe & R. N. Leaton (Eds.), Habituation: Perspectives from child development, animal behavior, and neurophysiology (pp. 297–320), Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum). Support for this theory, however, has been derived primarily from studies of retardate learning. Normal children's novelty preference was examined within a modified Moss-Harlow (E. Moss & H. F. Harlow, 1947, Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 40, 333–342) design to compare Zeaman's model with that of S. L. Witryol and W. Wanich (1983, The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 143, 3–8). Each of 16 problems, consisting of three single-stimulus demonstration trials and one two-choice test, was administered to 180 children (mean CA 4, 5.5, and 7 years) in three reward conditions. Novelty was manipulated by varying stimulus familiarization in the demonstration trials. Experiment 1 showed strong preferences for novel over familiar (demonstrated) stimuli at each age. Experiment 2 revealed novelty preference across age levels, two levels of reward contrast, and two levels of task difficulty. It was reasoned that Moss-Harlow tasks designed for normal children typically present a much higher level of difficulty than that intended by researchers. Furthermore, developmental decreases in novelty preference by retardates may derive from (a) transfer of training from prior experiments and (b) specific, repetitive instructions which may have directed attention away from stimulus novelty. 相似文献
298.
The influence of perceived somatotype on stereotypes of behavior associated with body build was investigated among 160 male and 140 female Nigerian children in secondary school. In both groups, the perception of subjects' own physiques and discrepancy between their perceived and preferred physiques significantly explained the variance in the character trait scores attributed to body types. In general, the subjects attributed positive character traits to their perceived somatotypes and undesirable traits to the physiques with which they were dissatisfied. Thus, the perception of somatotype and discrepancy between perceived and preferred physique could significantly differentiate the character traits attributed to body build among male and female children. 相似文献
299.
300.
Training and Research in Counselling 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
A. W. Bolger 《British Journal of Guidance & Counselling》1985,13(1):112-124
Formal counselling training in Britain began with the preparation of school counsellors in universities and relied largely upon American models. Early courses were highly didactic but over the last decade practical and experiential aspects have taken precedence. Basic counselling training has now been extended into the preparation of many workers, voluntary and professional. Counselling research also had academic origins, but has broadened to include many different work settings. Much of the published research has been in the form of surveys of counselling services, but some detailed studies of process are noted. There is a need for research to be increased, and the gap between research and practice shortened, if counselling is to demonstrate its relevance to contemporary needs. 相似文献