首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1853篇
  免费   94篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2023年   16篇
  2022年   10篇
  2021年   11篇
  2020年   39篇
  2019年   39篇
  2018年   64篇
  2017年   64篇
  2016年   60篇
  2015年   27篇
  2014年   59篇
  2013年   242篇
  2012年   83篇
  2011年   91篇
  2010年   49篇
  2009年   57篇
  2008年   66篇
  2007年   65篇
  2006年   68篇
  2005年   49篇
  2004年   66篇
  2003年   68篇
  2002年   55篇
  2001年   34篇
  2000年   27篇
  1999年   28篇
  1998年   30篇
  1997年   21篇
  1996年   26篇
  1995年   21篇
  1994年   16篇
  1993年   21篇
  1992年   24篇
  1991年   17篇
  1990年   17篇
  1989年   14篇
  1988年   17篇
  1987年   24篇
  1985年   29篇
  1984年   26篇
  1983年   13篇
  1982年   11篇
  1981年   13篇
  1980年   16篇
  1979年   13篇
  1978年   12篇
  1976年   13篇
  1974年   9篇
  1969年   9篇
  1967年   8篇
  1966年   9篇
排序方式: 共有1948条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
102.
Young and older adults provided language samples in response to elicitation questions while concurrently performing 3 different tasks. The language samples were scored on three dimensions: fluency, grammatical complexity, and content. Previous research had suggested the hypothesis that the restricted speech register of older adults is buffered from the costs of dual task demands. This hypothesis was tested by comparing language samples collected during a baseline condition with those produced while the participants were performing the concurrent tasks. The results indicate that young and older adults adopt different strategies when confronted with dual task demands. Young adults shift to a restricted speech register when confronted with dual task demands. Older adults, who were already using a restricted speech register, became less fluent although the grammatical complexity and informational content of their speech was preserved. Hence, some but not all aspects of older adults' speech are buffered from dual task demands.  相似文献   
103.
104.
105.
Counterfactual and prefactual conditionals.   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
We consider reasoning about prefactual possibilities in the future, for example, "if I were to win the lottery next year I would buy a yacht" and counterfactual possibilities, for example, "if I had won the lottery last year, I would have bought a yacht." People may reason about indicative conditionals, for example, "if I won the lottery I bought a yacht" by keeping in mind a few true possibilities, for example, "I won the lottery and I bought a yacht." They understand counterfactuals by keeping in mind two possibilities, the conjecture, "I won the lottery and I bought a yacht" and the presupposed facts, "I did not win the lottery and I did not buy a yacht." We report the results of three experiments on prefactuals that examine what people judge them to imply, the possibilities they judge to be consistent with them, and the inferences they judge to follow from them. The results show that reasoners keep a single possibility in mind to understand a prefactual.  相似文献   
106.
107.
We describe how PowerPoint presentation software can be used to create computer activity schedules to teach individuals with special needs. Presented are the steps involved in creating activity schedules with close-ended and open-ended activities, and for preparing schedules that include photos, sounds, text, and videos that can be used to occasion an individual's engagement in a variety of learning activities.  相似文献   
108.
Relations of overt and relational aggression with perceived popularity among children and early adolescents were examined in 2 studies (Ns = 607 and 1,049). Among older youths, positive concurrent relations found between overt aggression and perceived popularity became nonsignificant when relational aggression was controlled, whereas positive associations found between relational aggression and perceived popularity held when overt aggression was controlled. Aggression and perceived popularity were not positively related for the younger participants. The 2nd study also examined the temporal ordering of these relations over 6 months. For older girls, positive relations between relational aggression and perceived popularity were bidirectional. For older boys, relational aggression did not predict increased perceived popularity, but perceived popularity predicted increased relational aggression. Implications for intervention are discussed.  相似文献   
109.
110.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号