全文获取类型
收费全文 | 73983篇 |
免费 | 947篇 |
国内免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
74934篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 766篇 |
2019年 | 965篇 |
2018年 | 4239篇 |
2017年 | 3565篇 |
2016年 | 3342篇 |
2015年 | 1143篇 |
2014年 | 1037篇 |
2013年 | 4732篇 |
2012年 | 2192篇 |
2011年 | 4032篇 |
2010年 | 3401篇 |
2009年 | 2443篇 |
2008年 | 3198篇 |
2007年 | 3557篇 |
2006年 | 1467篇 |
2005年 | 1519篇 |
2004年 | 1447篇 |
2003年 | 1290篇 |
2002年 | 1261篇 |
2001年 | 1798篇 |
2000年 | 1717篇 |
1999年 | 1287篇 |
1998年 | 708篇 |
1997年 | 609篇 |
1996年 | 543篇 |
1995年 | 534篇 |
1994年 | 508篇 |
1993年 | 535篇 |
1992年 | 1023篇 |
1991年 | 970篇 |
1990年 | 956篇 |
1989年 | 857篇 |
1988年 | 825篇 |
1987年 | 815篇 |
1986年 | 808篇 |
1985年 | 873篇 |
1984年 | 683篇 |
1983年 | 586篇 |
1982年 | 481篇 |
1979年 | 659篇 |
1978年 | 502篇 |
1976年 | 464篇 |
1975年 | 597篇 |
1974年 | 632篇 |
1973年 | 605篇 |
1972年 | 545篇 |
1971年 | 481篇 |
1968年 | 555篇 |
1967年 | 556篇 |
1966年 | 489篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
931.
932.
Predicting contraceptive behavior from attitudes: a comparison of within- versus across-subjects procedures 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Within- versus across-subjects procedures for predicting behavior from attitudes were contrasted. Each procedure requires a comparison among attitudes in order to generate a prediction; the comparison is either among the same attitudes held by different people (across subjects) or among different attitudes held by the same person (within subject). It was hypothesized that the within-subject model provides a more adequate explanation of behavior from attitudinal constructs and, hence, more accurate prediction of behavior from attitudes than does the across-subjects model. To test this view, a sample of 349 married couples was administered a questionnaire containing measures of three attitudinal components--affect, cognition, and conation--toward each of four contraceptive methods--oral contraceptives, IUD, diaphragm, and condoms. Contraceptive behavior was assessed 1 year later. In support of the hypothesis, the within-subject predictions bore a significantly stronger relation to the behavioral criteria than did the across-subjects predictions. This effect was consistent for each of the three components of attitude, for the prediction of behavior and behavioral intention, for male and female respondents, and for a variety of contraceptive behaviors. In addition, both the within- and the across-subjects analyses demonstrated a clear rank ordering in the predictive validity of the three attitudinal components: Conation was a better predictor of behavior than was affect, which, in turn, was better than cognition. 相似文献
933.
J M Haviland T R McGuire P A Rothbaum 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1983,45(3):633-640
Plomin and Foch's (1980) study of objectively assessed personality in childhood is critiqued on five points: (a) conceptual validity of the measures, (b) stability of the measures for the population age range, (c) comparability of populations, (d) accuracy of literature review, and (e) appropriate interpretation of broad heritability data. The Plomin and Foch study contains major errors; it is theoretically and methodologically flawed. Their report is especially significant because it is representative of problems critical to the study of the genetic correlates of personality. 相似文献
934.
Reaction times of younger and older men and temporal contingencies of reinforcement 总被引:3,自引:3,他引:0
下载免费PDF全文

Influences of extended training and temporal contingencies on reaction time were studied in relation to developmental differences. Older and younger men were trained on a chained schedule in which completion of a variable interval produced a terminal link in which reaction time was measured. The reaction-time procedure involved a conditional discrimination with matching to sample in one component and oddity matching in the other. During baseline training, no time limit was placed on the response to the discrimination choice stimuli. Subsequently, increasingly severe time limits were imposed over a series of sessions. Older and younger men showed increased speeds (decreased reaction times) when temporal contingencies were imposed, and these changes were maintained during post-training baseline sessions when there was unlimited time in which to respond. The younger men generally responded faster than the older ones, and age differences were not appreciably reduced during the course of the experiment. The results indicated the feasibility of studying reaction time in human subjects using operant procedures analogous to those developed for the study of nonverbal organisms. 相似文献
935.
If spatial memory tasks are considered as foraging problems, it can be predicted that rats' difficulty in learning a win-stay task—revisiting part of a maze where food has recently been found—should be partly overcome if the food is not fully consumed at the first visit (nondepleted reward) rather than being all eaten (depleted reward). Three experiments confirmed and further analysed this result. Experiments 1 and 2 showed that it does not depend on the amount of food initially found; nor is win-stay performance affected by the amount of food actually eaten. Experiment 3 suggested that the effects of nondepleted reward are mainly due to the animals' being interrupted while eating, and very little to the fact that uneaten food is left behind. The results are discussed with regard to their implications for learning theory, and for the application of foraging theory to learning and memory experiments. 相似文献
936.
MARGARET A. CROUCH 《Metaphilosophy》1983,24(3):227-240
937.
John M. Kruse J.Bruce Overmier Wilbert A. Konz Eric Rokke 《Learning and motivation》1983,14(2):165-181
In a transfer-of-control experiment with rats, Pavlovian CSs were tested for the specificity of their effects. The instrumental behavior consisted of a discriminative, conditional two-lever choice task in which qualitatively different appetitive reinforcers were contingent upon the two correct choices. In a Pavlovian phase, subjects experienced conditioning to establish either a CS+ or CS? for one reinforcer or a CS+ or CS? for the other reinforcer. Finally, in a test, these CSs were presented when there was the opportunity to make choice responses. The CS+s evoked choices of the lever which had eventuated in the reinforcer that had served as the Pavlovian US, while the CS?s showed only a slight tendency to evoke the other choice responses. When the CSs were compounded with the original SDs, the CS+s had little effect upon the vigor of responding while the CS?s reduced the vigor of responding to the SD for the reinforcer that was the same as the US used in establishing the CS?. The results are discussed in terms of associative mediational theory and the reinforcer specificity of Pavlovian conditioned excitation and inhibition. 相似文献
938.
On the nature of intersensory facilitation of reaction time 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
939.
Although there were no significant differences in the single support phase of gait for 6 trainable mentally retarded boys (Mean age 9.6 yr.) and 6 nonretarded boys (Mean age 9.5 yr.), total gait time and time in double support were significantly different for the two groups, supporting previous research which showed gait was was deficient in trainable mentally retarded individuals. 相似文献
940.
A A Ashby 《Perceptual and motor skills》1983,57(2):649-650
The present experiment was designed to examine age-related characteristics of short-term retention of kinesthetic movement information. 20 children from each age group (6, 8, 10 yr.) were tested for recall of kinesthetic end-location. The results indicate that 6- and 8-yr.-old children became less accurate and more variable in recall performance following a delay interval. However, 10-yr.-old children were capable of maintaining performance accuracy during a delay interval. 相似文献