全文获取类型
收费全文 | 107162篇 |
免费 | 1931篇 |
国内免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
109101篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 1180篇 |
2019年 | 1461篇 |
2018年 | 1978篇 |
2017年 | 2024篇 |
2016年 | 2112篇 |
2015年 | 1506篇 |
2014年 | 1843篇 |
2013年 | 8615篇 |
2012年 | 3371篇 |
2011年 | 3461篇 |
2010年 | 2094篇 |
2009年 | 2156篇 |
2008年 | 2991篇 |
2007年 | 2922篇 |
2006年 | 2718篇 |
2005年 | 2348篇 |
2004年 | 2340篇 |
2003年 | 2212篇 |
2002年 | 2190篇 |
2001年 | 3218篇 |
2000年 | 3072篇 |
1999年 | 2330篇 |
1998年 | 1178篇 |
1997年 | 1031篇 |
1996年 | 1079篇 |
1995年 | 1025篇 |
1992年 | 1905篇 |
1991年 | 1798篇 |
1990年 | 1778篇 |
1989年 | 1720篇 |
1988年 | 1711篇 |
1987年 | 1571篇 |
1986年 | 1583篇 |
1985年 | 1710篇 |
1984年 | 1417篇 |
1983年 | 1299篇 |
1982年 | 1031篇 |
1979年 | 1514篇 |
1978年 | 1103篇 |
1976年 | 1005篇 |
1975年 | 1233篇 |
1974年 | 1413篇 |
1973年 | 1465篇 |
1972年 | 1249篇 |
1971年 | 1146篇 |
1970年 | 1021篇 |
1969年 | 1063篇 |
1968年 | 1311篇 |
1967年 | 1168篇 |
1966年 | 1126篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
871.
Generalization gradients of inhibition following auditory discrimination learning 总被引:3,自引:3,他引:0
下载免费PDF全文

A more direct method than the usual ones for obtaining inhibitory gradients requires that the dimension of the nonreinforced stimulus selected for testing be orthogonal to the dimensions of the reinforced stimulus. In that case, the test points along the inhibitory gradient are equally distant from the reinforced stimulus. An attempt was made to realize this condition by obtaining inhibitory gradients along the frequency dimension of a pure tone after discrimination training in which the nonreinforced stimulus was a pure tone (or tones), and the reinforced stimulus was either white noise or the absence of a tone. The results showed that some degree of specific inhibitory control was exerted by the frequency of the tone, although the gradients were broad and shallow in slope.
A further experiment was conducted to see whether the modification of an excitatory gradient resulting from training to discriminate neighboring tones could arise from a simple interaction of inhibitory and excitatory gradients. The results indicated that it could not, since discrimination training produced a concentration of responding in the vicinity of the reinforced stimulus which cannot be derived from any plausible gradient of inhibition.
相似文献872.
873.
874.
Shimp CP 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1967,10(1):57-65
When a pigeon's choices between two keys are probabilistically reinforced, as in discrete trial probability learning procedures and in concurrent variable-interval schedules, the bird tends to maximize, or to choose the alternative with the higher probability of reinforcement. In concurrent variable-interval schedules, steady-state matching, which is an approximate equality between the relative frequency of a response and the relative frequency of reinforcement of that response, has previously been obtained only as a consequence of maximizing. In the present experiment, maximizing was impossible. A choice of one of two keys was reinforced only if it formed, together with the three preceding choices, the sequence of four successive choices that had occurred least often. This sequence was determined by a Bernoulli-trials process with parameter p. Each of three pigeons matched when p was ½ or ¼. Therefore, steady-state matching by individual birds is not always a consequence of maximizing. Choice probability varied between successive reinforcements, and sequential statistics revealed dependencies which were adequately described by a Bernoulli-trials process with p depending on the time since the preceding reinforcement. 相似文献
875.
Schedules using noxious stimuli. II: low intensity electric shock as a discriminative stimulus
下载免费PDF全文

The presence or absence of pulses of low intensity electric shock was used as a discriminative stimulus to control responding under fixed ratio reinforcement in the squirrel monkey. Initially brief periods of nonreinforcement were lengthened only when discriminative control was evident. Discriminative control was studied by (1) varying the duration of nonreinforcement periods; (2) reversing the stimulus conditions correlated with reinforcement and nonreinforcement periods; and (3) determining the minimum shock intensity necessary to maintain discriminative control. Stimulus control was not reliably affected by d-amphetamine, chlorpromazine, or morphine. The discriminative control by pulses of low intensity electric shock was similar to that by other discriminative stimuli, except that the control developed slowly and was better when the pulsing shock was correlated with reinforcement than when correlated with nonreinforcement. 相似文献
876.
C P Shimp 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1967,10(5):425-434
Five contingencies were superimposed successively on a variable-interval schedule of reinforcement. In each of the resulting conditions, a different short, interresponse time was reinforced and an interresponse-time distribution was obtained from each of three pigeons. The lower bound of the reinforced interresponse times ranged from 0.3 to 2.4 sec. The resulting distributions were combined, according to a rationale based upon concurrent operants, induction, and a property of variable-interval schedules, to describe the interresponse-time distributions from a variable-interval schedule. 相似文献
877.
878.
Arm weight, adaptation,and weight discrimination 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
879.
880.