首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1477篇
  免费   19篇
  1496篇
  2021年   26篇
  2020年   16篇
  2019年   24篇
  2018年   30篇
  2017年   30篇
  2016年   40篇
  2015年   21篇
  2014年   31篇
  2013年   158篇
  2012年   47篇
  2011年   84篇
  2010年   31篇
  2009年   30篇
  2008年   48篇
  2007年   50篇
  2006年   33篇
  2005年   37篇
  2004年   56篇
  2003年   39篇
  2002年   28篇
  2001年   28篇
  2000年   25篇
  1999年   18篇
  1998年   24篇
  1997年   16篇
  1996年   18篇
  1995年   22篇
  1994年   21篇
  1993年   14篇
  1992年   23篇
  1991年   15篇
  1990年   19篇
  1989年   14篇
  1988年   21篇
  1987年   17篇
  1986年   11篇
  1985年   14篇
  1984年   16篇
  1983年   23篇
  1981年   14篇
  1980年   14篇
  1979年   19篇
  1978年   14篇
  1977年   16篇
  1976年   14篇
  1975年   14篇
  1974年   17篇
  1973年   15篇
  1967年   12篇
  1950年   13篇
排序方式: 共有1496条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
281.
ENTROPY AND EVIL     
Robert John Russell 《Zygon》1984,19(4):449-468
Abstract. This paper explores a possible relationship between entropy and evil in terms of metaphor. After presenting the various meanings of entropy in classical thermodynamics and statical mechanics, and the Augustinian and Irenaean theodicies, several similarities and dissimilarities between entropy and evil are described. Underlying the concepts of evil and entropy is the assumption that time has a direction. After examining the scientific basis for this assumption, it is hypothesized that, if evil is real in nature, entropy is what one would expect to find at the level of physical processes, and conversely that, if entropy is coupled to a physical arrow of time, one could expect to find dissipative yet catalytic processes in history and religious experience.  相似文献   
282.
283.
Sixty male subjects were either attacked or treated neutrally by a confederate, after which each saw a videotape of two men fighting. Subjects were informed that fight was either real or fictitious or were given no explanation of it. Subjects who had previously been attacked and had observed the fight under a set to perceive it as real were subsequently more punitive in their treatment of the confederate than subjects in all other conditions. The combination of prior attack and observation of real violence also sustained blood pressure (BP) at near the level produced by the attack, whereas BP of attacked subjects in the other conditions declined during the time the fight was observed. Palmar sweat measures revealed that observation of real violence was more arousing than observation of fictitious fighting. The results are discussed in terms of the effects that the reality of observed violence has on emotional arousal.  相似文献   
284.
Rats were familiarized to one compartment of a test box and then given access to this and a novel compartment. Females spent more time in the novel compartment than males did and this difference was found to be independent of a possible difference in familiarity with the familiar compartment arising from the females' higher frequency of ambulating and rearing with sniffing during familiarization. This is evidence of a sex difference in response to novelty independent of activity differences. Females also had shorter approach latencies to the novel compartment. These sex differences could reflect differences in either fearfulness or curiosity.  相似文献   
285.
286.
Pigeons peck faster during a signal for reward (S+) when that signal alternates with one for absence of reward (S-). This “contrast effect” has been shown to involve diminished preference for S + compared with a stimulus not involved in a discrimination. The present experiment demonstrates that the signal produced by pecks to S+ in a chained schedule is responded to in proportion to the contrast effect during S +. The result suggests that a prior interpretation of contrast, in terms of Amsel's frustration theory, is not the correct one.  相似文献   
287.
288.
289.
A model of categorical inference (Revlis, 1975b) claims that a conversion operation participates in the encoding of quantified, categorical expressions. As a consequence, a reasoner is said to interpret such sentences as “All A are B” in a way that permits it to also be the case that “All B are A.” The present study examines this conception of encoding using a sentence-picture verification task. In two experiments, students were asked to judge whether one of five possible Euler diagrams was true or false of a categorical expression (e.g., All A are B, No A are B, Some A are B, Some A are not B). Verification errors support a three-stage verification model whose major component is access to a “meaning stack” representing the progressive analysis of categorical relations; at the top of that stack is a converted reading of the input sentence. These findings have implications for current conceptions of categorical inference and semantic retrieval.  相似文献   
290.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号