首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1754篇
  免费   69篇
  国内免费   1篇
  1824篇
  2021年   30篇
  2020年   18篇
  2019年   37篇
  2018年   41篇
  2017年   47篇
  2016年   48篇
  2015年   26篇
  2014年   45篇
  2013年   190篇
  2012年   59篇
  2011年   106篇
  2010年   53篇
  2009年   46篇
  2008年   54篇
  2007年   66篇
  2006年   45篇
  2005年   50篇
  2004年   67篇
  2003年   45篇
  2002年   38篇
  2001年   35篇
  2000年   29篇
  1999年   22篇
  1998年   26篇
  1997年   17篇
  1996年   20篇
  1995年   24篇
  1994年   24篇
  1993年   15篇
  1992年   24篇
  1991年   19篇
  1990年   23篇
  1989年   19篇
  1988年   28篇
  1987年   20篇
  1986年   13篇
  1985年   18篇
  1984年   19篇
  1983年   24篇
  1981年   16篇
  1980年   14篇
  1979年   19篇
  1978年   18篇
  1977年   16篇
  1976年   16篇
  1975年   15篇
  1974年   17篇
  1973年   14篇
  1967年   13篇
  1950年   13篇
排序方式: 共有1824条查询结果,搜索用时 7 毫秒
201.
We show that if overall sample size and effect size are held constant, the power of theF test for a one-way analysis of variance decreases dramatically as the number of groups increases. This reduction in power is even greater when the groups added to the design do not produce treatment effects. If a second independent variable is added to the design, either a split-plot or a completely randomized design may be employed. For the split-plot design, we show that the power of theF test on the betweengroups factor decreases as the correlation across the levels of the within-groups factor increases. The attenuation in between-groups power becomes more pronounced as the number of levels of the withingroups factor increases. Sample size and total cost calculations are required to determine whether the split-plot or completely randomized design is more efficient in a particular application. The outcome hinges on the cost of obtaining (or recruiting) a single subject relative to the cost of obtaining a single observation: We call this thesubject-to-observation cost (SOC) ratio. Split-plot designs are less costly than completely randomized designs only when the SOC ratio is high, the correlation across the levels of the within-groups factor is low, and the number of such levels is small.  相似文献   
202.
The accuracy of the two-moment, three-moment, square root, and cube root approximations to the noncentralF distribution was assessed using 7,920 entries from Tiku’s (1967) power tables. Tiku’s tables list exact values of β for α= .005?.05, ν1 = 1?12, ν2 = 2?120, and ?= 0.5?3.0. Analysis of the errors showed generally satisfactory performance for all four approximations. The three-moment approximation was most accurate, registering a maximum error of only .009. The other three approximations had maximum errors of ±.02, except for the square root approximation at ν2 = 2, where maximum errors of .05 occur. Approximation error increased with decreases in ν1 and, less consistently, with increases in ν2. Error was nonmonotonically related to ?. A second investigation explored the accuracy of the approximations at values of αranging from .10 to .90. All four approximations degraded substantially in this situation, with maximum errors ranging from ?.09 to .05. If the analysis is restricted to cases where ν1 τ; 1 and ν2 τ; 2, maximum errors drop to roughly ±.03. We conclude that the approximations perform reasonably well for small αand moderately well for larger values, if certain restrictions are imposed. From a computational standpoint, however, there is little advantage to using approximate as opposed to exact methods unless exact values ofF α are known in advance.  相似文献   
203.
204.
205.
A comparison is made between the Vocational Development Inventory and the Readiness for Career Planning scale when both were administered to the same sample. An analysis of covariance, with intelligence controlled for, indicated that both instruments show an over-all progression in vocational maturity at different grade levels, as well as differences between socioeconomic groups. A correlational matrix found vocational maturity scores from the two instruments to be significantly related.  相似文献   
206.
207.
The difference threshold for detection of barriers at different distances using a modified method of limits was found, for four blinded rats, to be 9 in. White noise 20 dB above ambient level reduced discrimination performance to chance, indicating that auditory cues were involved in the discrimination. A second determination, utilizing the method of constant stimuli, confirmed the difference threshold to be 9 in. and reduced individual S variation by further training. Evidence with a black and a white barrier indicated the possibility that infrared heat cues do not enter into the discrimination.  相似文献   
208.
Predictions from Maier's theory of “frustration”-instigated behaviour have been tested in an experimental situation differing significantly from that in which the theory was propounded yet containing the central element of “frustration”—the insoluble problem.

A water discrimination unit was employed in which the performance of rats would be observed during attacks on insoluble problems, position problems or symbol problems.

Two groups, each containing ten Wistar albino rats, served as subjects. The research design consisted of the following phases: preliminary training, development of position responses, exposure to a symbol-reward problem with 50 per cent, punishment and exposure to a symbol-reward problem with 100 per cent, punishment. The design differed for the two groups only at the phase in which the position responses were established. During this phase one group was exposed to a position-reward problem and the other to an insoluble problem.

Position responses were established as frequently under position-“frustration” (position stereotypes) as under position-reward (position habits) conditions. Position stereotypes were more rigid—more resistant to extinction—than position habits under conditions of 50 per cent, punishment. Position stereotypes were as readily extinguished under 100 per cent, punishment as were position habits under 30 per cent, punishment.

The first two observations conform to predictions made from Maier's theory. The third does not. That is to say, not all situations containing the basic elements of “frustration” give rise to stereotyped behaviour patterns which are as rigid or “fixated” as Maier's theory would predict. It is a reasonable hypothesis that the characteristics of stereotyped responses established in certain “frustration” situations may be described adequately in terms of conventional learning principles without the necessity of resorting to a distinction between “goal-motivated” and “frustration-instigated” behaviour.  相似文献   
209.
210.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号