首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   822篇
  免费   61篇
  国内免费   71篇
  954篇
  2024年   3篇
  2023年   17篇
  2022年   26篇
  2021年   24篇
  2020年   28篇
  2019年   36篇
  2018年   49篇
  2017年   41篇
  2016年   52篇
  2015年   29篇
  2014年   29篇
  2013年   114篇
  2012年   54篇
  2011年   55篇
  2010年   36篇
  2009年   27篇
  2008年   48篇
  2007年   41篇
  2006年   47篇
  2005年   36篇
  2004年   26篇
  2003年   14篇
  2002年   24篇
  2001年   4篇
  2000年   8篇
  1999年   10篇
  1998年   6篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   8篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   6篇
  1993年   5篇
  1992年   4篇
  1991年   4篇
  1989年   5篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   4篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   2篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   3篇
  1980年   2篇
  1975年   1篇
  1974年   1篇
  1973年   1篇
  1971年   1篇
排序方式: 共有954条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
921.
Two lexical decision experiments were conducted to determine whether there is a specific, localized influence of the item frequency of consecutive trials (i.e., first-order sequential effects) when the trials are not related to each other. Both low-frequency words and nonwords were influenced by the frequency of the precursor word (Experiment 1). In contrast, high-frequency words showed little sensitivity to the frequency of the precursor word (Experiment 2), although they showed longer reaction times for word trials preceded by a nonword trial. The presence of sequential effects in the lexical decision task suggests that participants shift their response criteria on a trial-by-trial basis.  相似文献   
922.
The time course of a bias in predicting danger as a function of a repressive coping style was examined. The participants belonged to repressor, low - anxious, or high - anxious groups. They read context sentences predictive of threat or nonthreat event outcomes, followed by target words for rapid naming representing the outcomes. The interval between context and target word was 50 ms (Experiment 1), 550 ms (Experiment 2), or 1050 ms (Experiments 3 and 4). There was bias for naming words confirming threatening outcomes: (a) for repressors in the 550-ms delay condition; and (b) for high - anxiety participants in the 1050-ms delay condition. Repressive coping facilitated early processing of threat, but inhibited late processing, whereas high anxiety was characterised by sustained vigilance for threat. There were clear differences in threat processing by repressors and the low - anxious participants  相似文献   
923.
新型冠状病毒肺炎疫情下普通民众焦虑的影响因素研究   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
甄瑞  周宵 《应用心理学》2020,(2):99-107
为考察新型冠状病毒肺炎疫情下普通民众焦虑情绪的影响因素,采用自陈式问卷,对新型冠状病毒肺炎疫情防控下的1050位普通民众的疫情经历、媒体信息暴露时长及性质、疫情风险感知和焦虑进行调查。结果发现新型冠状病毒肺炎疫情防控当下民众焦虑情绪的发生率为18.0%,焦虑情绪的发生率在性别、年龄、受教育程度、婚姻状况和月收入等方面没有显著的差异;疫情经历、媒体信息暴露时长、负面信息暴露和感知疫情的严重性会增加焦虑情绪产生的可能性,正面信息暴露、对疫情熟悉度和可控度的感知可以减少焦虑情绪产生的可能性。这些结果说明了疫情经历、媒体信息暴露时长和性质、疫情风险感知是疫情防控当下民众焦虑出现的重要因素。  相似文献   
924.
925.
芮执俭 《管子学刊》2005,3(3):111-113
“六峜”在中国古籍中仅见于《管子》.对“六峜”一词的解释,近、现代研究《管子》的学者均释为“六法”或“八卦”.本文对“六峜”提出新的释义,指出“六峜”实际上是中国远古时代“天文始祖”伏羲在“立周天历度”中所创造的对八卦的一种运用方式.  相似文献   
926.
927.
李锐 《周易研究》2007,1(3):28-33
本文从陈鼓应先生论证“《易传》道家说”的重点文章《〈彖传〉与老庄》、《〈彖传〉的道家思维方式》出发,不仅指出其论据方面的不足,而且指出其论证方法是使用“丐辞”、“默证”。因此,《彖传》不能成立。甚至陈先生所用的“道家”这一名目,也不适当。从而说明“《易传》道家说”不能成立,其“道家主干说”也因此缺少重要文献材料,难以服人。  相似文献   
928.
李锐 《周易研究》2004,(4):57-60
本文讨论帛书<易传>第一篇<二三子问>的时代问题.依据传世文献材料和出土文献,并根据思想观念的发展规律,指出<二三子问>中出现的"精白"一词,已见于<鹃冠子·度万>,进而分析在孔子的时代,有可能出现"精白"这样的概念以及出现押韵的占语,对于<二三子问>的年代作出了新的推测.认为此篇时代当较早,并没有受到黄老思想的影响,有可能就是孔门弟子录所闻于夫子,整理成篇.  相似文献   
929.
Four experiments were carried out to investigate how general knowledge about the stereotypical gender of participants in a text influences comprehension. A self-paced reading task was used to present short texts comprising one, two, or three sentences. The first sentence of each text introduced a stereotypically masculine or feminine participant (e.g. doctor, nurse), or a neutral one. The last sentence introduced a pronoun (he/she) that could match or mismatch the gender of the referent. The first experiment, which was carried out in English, showed that reading times for the last sentence were longer when there was a mismatch than when there was a match between the gender of the pronoun in the last sentence and the stereotypical gender of the referent in the first sentence. In contrast to English, the gender of the participant can be disambiguated by a preceding article ( el / la ) in Spanish. The results of the second, third, and fourth experiments, which were carried out in Spanish, showed that reading times for the first sentences were longer when there was a mismatch than when there was a match between the gender of the article and the stereotypical gender of the participant. However, reading times for the last sentences did not differ. Overall, the results suggest that information about the stereotypical gender of the participants in a text is incorporated into the representation as soon as it becomes available, and that it affects the ease with which the text is understood.  相似文献   
930.
Abstract

Anxiety often impairs performance of “difficult” tasks (especially under test conditions), but there are numerous exceptions. Theories of anxiety and performance need to address at least two major issues: (1) the complexity and apparent inconsistency of the findings; and (2) the conceptual definition of task difficulty. Some theorists (e.g. Humphreys & Revelle, 1984; Sarason, 1988) argue that anxiety causes worry, and worry always impairs performance on tasks with high attentional or short-term memory demands. According to the processing efficiency theory, worry has two main effects: (1) a reduction in the storage and processing capacity of the working memory system available for a concurrent task; and (2) an increment in on-task effort and activities designed to improve performance. There is a crucial distinction within the theory between performance effectiveness (= quality of performance) and processing efficiency (= performance effectiveness divided by effort). Anxiety characteristically impairs efficiency more than effectiveness.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号