首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1073篇
  免费   35篇
  2021年   9篇
  2020年   15篇
  2019年   18篇
  2018年   31篇
  2017年   22篇
  2016年   29篇
  2015年   15篇
  2014年   21篇
  2013年   119篇
  2012年   40篇
  2011年   37篇
  2010年   31篇
  2009年   25篇
  2008年   34篇
  2007年   39篇
  2006年   29篇
  2005年   30篇
  2004年   33篇
  2003年   28篇
  2002年   33篇
  2001年   19篇
  2000年   14篇
  1999年   15篇
  1997年   10篇
  1996年   12篇
  1995年   10篇
  1994年   11篇
  1991年   9篇
  1990年   10篇
  1989年   11篇
  1988年   12篇
  1986年   12篇
  1985年   10篇
  1984年   19篇
  1983年   14篇
  1982年   11篇
  1981年   17篇
  1980年   9篇
  1979年   10篇
  1977年   12篇
  1976年   12篇
  1975年   15篇
  1974年   21篇
  1973年   13篇
  1972年   11篇
  1970年   10篇
  1969年   14篇
  1968年   10篇
  1967年   16篇
  1966年   15篇
排序方式: 共有1108条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
This study evaluated the relative efficacy of long (1 minute) and short (15 seconds) durations of negative practice for reducing self-stimulatory leg pounding of a traumatically brain-injured adult. In individual social skill training classes, long negative practice and short negative practice were administered concurrently in an alternating treatments design. Generalization of treatment effects outside of training sessions was assessed during the client's lunch period. The results revealed that both durations of negative practice were equally effective in reducing leg pounding during training sessions. However, the effects of training did not generalize to settings in which the practice contingency was not enforced. Pounding was more frequent and negative practice somewhat less effective during periods of high social stimulation.  相似文献   
12.
The performance of poor and normal sixth-grade readers was compared on an auditory short-term memory task in two studies. In the first study, the effects of distraction, list length, and speed of stimulus presentation were investigated to test the hypothesis that the performance of the poor readers is affected by deficits in selective attention. Group differences in performance, however, were obtained for both distraction and no-distraction conditions. The second experiment examined the hypothesis that group differences may reflect differences in the use of rehearsal. In the second experiment, half the reading-disabled children and half the controls were provided with rehearsal training prior to completing the same short-term memory task a second time. Rehearsal training resulted in improved performance relative to children who were not trained; however, there was no rehearsal training x reading group interaction. The poor readers continued to show deficits in performance relative to the performance of control children. The results of these two studies suggest that deficits observed for poor readers on short-term memory tasks may reflect processing difficulties in some area that precedes rehearsal and affects performance under both distraction and no-distraction conditions.The authors wish to express their thanks to the Sachem School District and Betty Gould, Nicholas Opinanate, and Guy Barber for their unlimited cooperation, and to John Neale for very helpful discussions of a number of issues in this paper.  相似文献   
13.
Two studies tested the effects of TV ads with celebrity endorsement on the product preference and understanding of 8- to 14-year-old boys. Study 1 compared two ads for a model racer. One had celebrity endorsement (by a famous race driver) and footage of real automobile racing featuring the celebrity (live action); the second had neither feature. Study 2 employed one ad for a different brand of model racer edited to generate a 2 (endorser presence) by 2 (inclusion of live racetrack action) factorial design. A total of 415 boys were exposed to one of the experimental ads or a control ad, embedded in a new animated children's adventure program. Preference for the advertised brand of model racer (pre- and postviewing) and a number of cognitive variables were assessed. Exposure to endorsement led to increased preference for the toy and belief that the celebrity was expert about the toy. Live action led to exaggerated estimates of the physical properties of the toy and the belief that the ad was not staged. The 8- to 10-year-olds associated the glamour of the endorser with the toy and were more reliant on his advice than were 11- to 14-year-olds. However, the two age groups were not differentially affected by the ads. Contrary to the speculation of many researchers, understanding about advertising intent and techniques and cynicism about ads had almost no influence on product preference after viewing.  相似文献   
14.
The influence of contextual stimuli on the conditioning and performance of responding to a discrete stimulus was examined in the US preexposure paradigm using both context shift manipulations and a measure of context conditioning. Four groups of rats received both repeated exposure to an electric shock US in one context (Context 1), and repeated nonshocked exposure to a second context (Context 2). Two additional groups of rats received exposure to these contexts, but never received shock presentations. Rats exposed to shock learned to escape from the stimuli of Context 1, but did not escape from the stimuli provided by Context 2. Rats not exposed to shock failed to escape from either context. All rats then received a single CER conditioning session in which four pairings of a 3-min noise CS and shock US were presented. Half the rats received those CS-US pairings in the excitatory Context 1, while the remaining rats received those pairings in the neutral Context 2. Finally, half the rats in each of the CER conditioning treatments received extinction test trials of the noise CS in Context 1, while the remaining rats received those test trials in Context 2. Thus, this design factorially manipulated the presence of excitatory or neutral contextual stimuli during both conditioning and testing of a discrete CS. In comparison with the two groups of rats never preexposed to shock alone, attenuation in acquisition of conditioned suppression observed during test trials occurred only when CER conditioning had been administered in the excitatory Context 1, and this effect was manifested when testing occurred in either the excitatory Context 1 or the neutral Context 2. These results support the model of R. A. Rescorla and A. R. Wagner (1972) (in A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.) Classical Conditioning II, pp. 64–99, New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts) which asserts that contextual stimuli and sicrete CSs compete for limited associative strength supportable by a given US.  相似文献   
15.
On a lu en anglais un court passage de prose à difféients groupes d'étudiants ghanéens et new yorkais. Ensuite, et par trois fois, on leur demande de dire ce qu'ils ont retenu. Le premier rappel a lieu après 30–45 minutes, le second et le troisiàme après plusieurs semaines. On a utilisé trois histoires différentes, mais chaque groupe de sujets n'en entendait qu'une seule. Les résultats montrent que, conformément à L'idée que la tradition orale est plus forte en Afrique Occidentale qu'à New York, les étudiants ghanéens retenaient mieux que les américains. Pour deux des histoires, on a enregistré peu de perte de rétention des thèmes entre le 1er et le 3e rappel; pour la troisiéme, entre le 2e et le 3e rappel. Mais la capacité à reteñir les thèmes de L'histoire ne s'explique pas uniquement par puré routine de mémoire. On peut penser que L'ordonnancement de L'histoire, loin de constituer un obstacle comme c'est souvent le cas quand on doit retenir dans L'ordre des faits sans liaison entre eux, favorisait ici la rétention des thèmes.  相似文献   
16.
17.
18.
Arm weight, adaptation,and weight discrimination   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
  相似文献   
19.
20.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号