全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3774篇 |
免费 | 145篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
3922篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 17篇 |
2021年 | 18篇 |
2020年 | 58篇 |
2019年 | 72篇 |
2018年 | 100篇 |
2017年 | 99篇 |
2016年 | 101篇 |
2015年 | 67篇 |
2014年 | 83篇 |
2013年 | 465篇 |
2012年 | 145篇 |
2011年 | 140篇 |
2010年 | 92篇 |
2009年 | 94篇 |
2008年 | 125篇 |
2007年 | 139篇 |
2006年 | 128篇 |
2005年 | 130篇 |
2004年 | 129篇 |
2003年 | 115篇 |
2002年 | 121篇 |
2001年 | 74篇 |
2000年 | 54篇 |
1999年 | 71篇 |
1998年 | 64篇 |
1997年 | 39篇 |
1996年 | 69篇 |
1995年 | 60篇 |
1994年 | 59篇 |
1993年 | 54篇 |
1992年 | 66篇 |
1991年 | 39篇 |
1990年 | 47篇 |
1989年 | 49篇 |
1988年 | 44篇 |
1987年 | 49篇 |
1986年 | 43篇 |
1985年 | 67篇 |
1984年 | 43篇 |
1983年 | 53篇 |
1982年 | 44篇 |
1981年 | 35篇 |
1980年 | 36篇 |
1979年 | 33篇 |
1978年 | 32篇 |
1977年 | 24篇 |
1976年 | 20篇 |
1975年 | 23篇 |
1974年 | 28篇 |
1973年 | 22篇 |
排序方式: 共有3922条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
81.
In three experiments, rats learned to respond differentially to reinforced and nonreinforced trials when the length of the intertrial interval (ITI) predicted the trial outcome (reinforcement or nonreinforcement). Rats in control groups, for whom the length of the ITI did not predict the outcome, did not show differential performance on nonreinforced and reinforced trials. Generalization gradients obtained following discrimination training were comparable to those obtained following discrimination training with other types of discriminative stimuli. That is, groups which had shown differential performance in discrimination training yielded generalization gradients with fastest speeds at the previously reinforced ITIs, slowest speeds at the previously nonreinforced ITIs, and intermediate speeds at ITIs of intermediate length. Control groups yielded flat gradients across all ITIs tested. These effects were shown for relative time discrimination (short time = reinforcement, long time = nonreinforcement, or the reverse) and also for an absolute time discrimination (long and short time = reinforcement, middle time = nonreinforcement or the reverse). This method was effective for time duration measured in minutes rather than seconds, as is more commonly the case. 相似文献
82.
A schema-based theory of music perception that describes the dynamic interaction between the musical event and the listener’s knowledge of the underlying regularities in tonal music is proposed. Three properties of musical schema are evaluated in a recognition memory experiment: (1) The schema engages a subset of the abstract knowledge system that is determined by the predominant key of the musical sequence, (2) the schema evaluates both interval relations and the functions of the sounded elements within the established tonal framework, and (3) the schema interacts continuously with the musical event in time to process pitch information in its temporal context. Listeners are required to identify the serial position of a chord that is changed between two successive chord sequences that are otherwise identical. The experiment measures the magnitude and the temporal extent of the disruptive effect of including in the sequence an element outside the tonal framework. The results show temporally specific effects on memory for pitch relations consistent with the operation of a musical schema. Comparisons are made with schema-based theories applied in other perceptual and cognitive domains. 相似文献
83.
Valerie Tate Angel C.S.W. Carole Katz C.S.W. Arlene Litwack M.S. Mary Maffia Ph.D. Robert A. Mednick Ph.D. Richard Mingoia C.S.W. Rene Rocha Ph.D. 《Group》1983,7(3):55-61
This paper reflects the working through of a group's fantasy that the deceased group leader wished the group to continue. As in any defense, the fantasy protected members from the pain of the finality of the leader's death. This article reveals the subjective experience concerning the effect of the leader's death on the group and members' intrapsychic struggle to separate and individuate from the deceased leader, while attending to and striving toward the group's continuation. The group's painful and growing individuation enabled the group to follow its own tasks without the spector of the deceased leader. 相似文献
84.
Linda Rose Krasnor 《Journal of applied developmental psychology》1983,4(1):81-98
An observationally based assessment of social problem-solving skill was contrasted with more traditional testing methods. A preschool child with low social success was chosen for a demonstration profile analysis. The relative frequencies of the social goals, strategies, and targets shown by this child during free play were plotted in relation to that of his same-sex peer group. Parallel profiles of his relative success with specific goals, strategies, and targets were also constructed and compared to relevant group means and standard deviations. Specific social problem-solving strengths and weaknesses were identified from these profiles, using both intra- and interindividual analyses. The benefits and limitations of this approach for measurement and treatment of social problem-solving deficits were discussed. 相似文献
85.
Somatic complaints in anxious children 总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6
Deborah C. Beidel Mary Anne G. Christ Patrician J. Long 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1991,19(6):659-670
Although stomachaches and headaches are considered characteristic of children with anxiety disorders, there is converging evidence that a broader range of somatic symptoms may be associated with children's expressions of anxiety. The purpose of this study was to determine the prevalence of somatic complaints in anxious children. The results indicated that children with anxiety disorders endorsed the presence of many different somatic complaints, and that contrary to clinical intuition, stomaches and headaches were not among the most commonly reported symptoms. In addition, the anxious children endorsed significantly more somatic complaints when compared to normal controls. Furthermore, the symptom pattern reported by anxious children indicated the presence of both the somatic and cognitive components usually associated with panic attacks, although none of the children met diagnostic criteria for panic disorder. The results are discussed in terms of the contribution of somatic symptoms to the understanding of anxiety disorders in children. 相似文献
86.
We investigated differences between mildly depressed subjects and normal controls in their memory for, and judgments of, another person. All subjects read a story (containing either predominately positive or predominately negative items) under instructions to form an impression of the story target. Subjects later made several judgments about the target and attempted to recall the story. For both the positive and the negative story, depressed subjects produced significantly more negative intrusions (i.e., recalled nonpresented negative items) than did the normal subjects, but they did not recall more negative items correctly than did the normal subjects. Depressed subjects also perceived themselves as more similar to the negative story target than did the normal controls and indicated greater liking for the negative story target than did the normal controls. 相似文献
87.
Attractiveness ratings of 50 children aged 12–38 months were investigated in relation to adult socializing agents' behavior toward the children in their homes and play groups and their attitudes toward the children. Attractiveness scores were derived from 43 judges' ratings of facial photographs of the children. The same children were observed in play groups with female care givers; 40 of them were also observed in their homes with their parents. Play-group care givers rated the children for likability, tractability, and peer relations. Parents rated their attitudes toward giving routine care to their child. Attractiveness scores were not correlated with any adult attitude measures. Girls' attractiveness scores were positively correlated with their attempts to communicate in home and play group, and with mothers', fathers', and care givers' instructional responses, but negatively correlated with passive behavior in the home and with mothers' positive reactions. Boys' attractiveness scores were negatively correlated with passive behavior in the play group; no other significant correlations were found in the data for boys. 相似文献
88.
89.
The language patterns of 10 mothers and 10 fathers with their firstborn 3-year-old sons were compared. Ten-minute mother-son and father-son conversations were recorded independently in the home during a free-play session with standard play mterials. The findings of this study indicate that although mothers and fathers used a similar number of utterances in conversing with their sons over a 10-minute period, their patterns of speech were different on a number of quantitative measures including MLU, MLU of five longest utterances, percent of imperatives, and percent of questions. In general, fathers' communication was viewed as more controlling and involving the child less, while mothers' communication was viewed as more child-centered and involving the child more. 相似文献
90.