全文获取类型
收费全文 | 28432篇 |
免费 | 1072篇 |
国内免费 | 15篇 |
专业分类
29519篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 258篇 |
2020年 | 371篇 |
2019年 | 395篇 |
2018年 | 616篇 |
2017年 | 651篇 |
2016年 | 621篇 |
2015年 | 409篇 |
2014年 | 498篇 |
2013年 | 2446篇 |
2012年 | 1056篇 |
2011年 | 1105篇 |
2010年 | 691篇 |
2009年 | 568篇 |
2008年 | 939篇 |
2007年 | 837篇 |
2006年 | 780篇 |
2005年 | 694篇 |
2004年 | 588篇 |
2003年 | 546篇 |
2002年 | 583篇 |
2001年 | 779篇 |
2000年 | 824篇 |
1999年 | 602篇 |
1998年 | 280篇 |
1997年 | 238篇 |
1992年 | 483篇 |
1991年 | 432篇 |
1990年 | 466篇 |
1989年 | 439篇 |
1988年 | 421篇 |
1987年 | 417篇 |
1986年 | 418篇 |
1985年 | 438篇 |
1984年 | 347篇 |
1983年 | 312篇 |
1982年 | 249篇 |
1979年 | 399篇 |
1978年 | 289篇 |
1977年 | 256篇 |
1976年 | 234篇 |
1975年 | 350篇 |
1974年 | 383篇 |
1973年 | 360篇 |
1972年 | 340篇 |
1971年 | 295篇 |
1970年 | 267篇 |
1969年 | 330篇 |
1968年 | 367篇 |
1967年 | 327篇 |
1966年 | 332篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 62 毫秒
101.
G I Kempen 《Psychological reports》1992,70(2):608-610
Researchers concluded that Item 2 of the Short-form General Health Survey of the Medical Outcomes Study can be used as a single-item measure of health-related quality of life. This conclusion is tested again on a random sample of elderly people in the Netherlands. Although it can be concluded that the psychometric properties of the short form are satisfactory, the use of the single item as a measure of health-related quality of life is discussed. 相似文献
102.
The influence of a videotape, shown in a hospital admitting room, on patients' state anxiety and concerns about hospitalization was assessed in a preliminary study. For both state anxiety and specific concerns regarding hospitalization the pretest scores on each variable accounted for the preponderance of the variance in the posttest scores. In both instances, the intervention and the interaction of the intervention with the pretest scores accounted for less than 1% of variance in the outcome. While finding small effects to be significant for such a small sample (N = 93) is unlikely, the sample size was adequate to detect medium to large effects. More important was the fact that 73.33% of the videotape intervention group indicated that they did not watch the video, which leads us to the conclusion that this intervention as tested is not worthwhile. 相似文献
103.
Most studies of work-related Type A behavior have been conducted with samples of white men. Using a mail questionnaire to assess Type A behavior, job satisfaction, and daily stress and tension among 250 black men and 233 black women public accountants, the prevalence of Type A behavior and its relationship with satisfaction and stress were highly consistent with those reported for other samples of white men and women. 相似文献
104.
G R Foxall 《Psychological reports》1992,70(1):169-170
Differences between the scores of male and female respondents, 115 British, 123 Australian, and 131 American midcareer MBA students, confirm propositions derived from Kirton's adaption-innovation theory to the effect that individuals who diverge most from social norms will be among the most innovative members of their group. 相似文献
105.
This study examined the effect of demographic variables and intellectual factors on the Short-term Memory Test. Subjects were 20 patients neurologically diagnosed as brain-damaged. There was only one significant correlation between Verbal IQ and the Short-term Memory Test. Demographic variables of age, education, and sex did not correlate significantly with scores of the Short-term Memory Test. 相似文献
106.
G L Borofsky 《Psychological reports》1992,70(2):563-592
This paper summarizes a number of studies in which the validity of the Employee Reliability Inventory, a preemployment screening instrument designed to assess the likelihood of reliable and productive workplace behavior, was examined. Criterion-related studies compared the scores of a broadly diverse group of job applicants with those obtained from an array of criterion and comparison groups, for whom there was documented evidence of reliable or unreliable behavior. Criterion-related evidence indicates that the six scales are effective in differentiating a variety of criterion groups with unreliable behavior from a number of different job applicant comparison groups. Construct-related evidence for the validity of an emotional adjustment scale is reported as well. The issue of response distortion in preemployment inventories is discussed, and data are reported which indicate that scores on all six scales appear to be functionally free from the potentially confounding effects of response distortion. These results are consistent with the original validation and cross-validation findings, which supported the validity of the six scales when assessing the likelihood of reliable behavior in a population of job applicants. 相似文献
107.
108.
Functional models of face recognition and speech production have developed separately. However, naming a familiar face is, of course, an act of speech production. In this paper we propose a revision of Bruce and Young's (1986) model of face processing, which incorporates two features of Levelt's (1989) model of speech production. In particular, the proposed model includes two stages of lexical access for names and monitoring of face naming based on a "perceptual loop". Two predictions were derived from the perceptual loop hypothesis of speech monitoring: (1) naming errors in which a (correct) rare surname is erroneously replaced by a common surname should occur more frequently than the reverse substitution (the error asymmetry effect); (2) naming errors in which a common surname is articulated are more likely to be repaired than errors which result in articulation of a rare surname (the error-repairing effect). Both predictions were supported by an analysis of face naming errors in a laboratory face naming task. In a further experiment we considered the possibility that the effects of surname frequency observed in face naming errors could be explained by the frequency sensitivity of lexical access in speech production. However, no effect of the frequency of the surname of the faces used in the previous experiment was found on face naming latencies. Therefore, it is concluded that the perceptual loop hypothesis provides the more parsimonious account of the entire pattern of the results. 相似文献
109.
In this paper we study the acquisition of German noun plurals in relation to the question of how children represent regular and irregular inflection. Pinker and Prince (1992) have demonstrated several dissociations between regular and irregular inflection in the English past tense system. However, in English, the default status of -ed is confounded with its high frequency; therefore inflectional systems other than English past tense formation must be examined. The noun plural system in German is particularly interesting, because most nouns have irregular plurals in German and the regular (default) plural is less frequent than several of the irregular plurals. Thus it is unclear how a language learner determines whether German even has a regular plural, and if so what form it takes. Based on longitudinal data from impaired and unimpaired monolingual German-speaking children, we find a striking, statistically significant correlation: plural affixes that are used in overregularizations, namely -n or -s, are left out within compounds. This correlation shows that even impaired children are sensitive to the distinction between regular and irregular morphology. We propose a linguistic analysis of the correlation in terms of Kiparsky's (1982, 1985) level-ordering model plus an additional ordering condition on affixes: default (regular) affixes cannot serve as input to compounding processes. 相似文献
110.