首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1823篇
  免费   62篇
  2020年   28篇
  2018年   26篇
  2017年   32篇
  2016年   34篇
  2015年   33篇
  2014年   38篇
  2013年   188篇
  2012年   71篇
  2011年   51篇
  2010年   32篇
  2009年   28篇
  2008年   57篇
  2007年   58篇
  2006年   51篇
  2005年   52篇
  2004年   48篇
  2003年   44篇
  2002年   58篇
  2001年   32篇
  2000年   23篇
  1999年   24篇
  1998年   29篇
  1997年   31篇
  1996年   35篇
  1995年   36篇
  1994年   31篇
  1993年   23篇
  1992年   27篇
  1991年   25篇
  1990年   27篇
  1989年   27篇
  1988年   17篇
  1987年   23篇
  1986年   20篇
  1985年   30篇
  1984年   34篇
  1983年   32篇
  1982年   35篇
  1981年   27篇
  1980年   36篇
  1979年   30篇
  1978年   28篇
  1977年   39篇
  1976年   40篇
  1975年   22篇
  1974年   40篇
  1973年   27篇
  1972年   12篇
  1969年   16篇
  1967年   13篇
排序方式: 共有1885条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
221.
The authors collected information from caregivers, trained observers, and parents to investigate quality elements in child-care programs designed for young children in center-based settings. Participants were 75 parents of children aged 15 to 36 months and their caregivers from 13 child-care centers in a southeastern state. Observers collected indicators of program quality and process and structural quality indicators, including adult-child ratio, group size, use of planned activities, use of child-designated space, housekeeping activities, and caregiver-child interactions. Participants responded to questions regarding their child-rearing beliefs, social support networks, perceived stress levels, and demographic characteristics. The best predictors of higher quality care and sensitive caregiver-child interaction in centers were specialized caregiver training, higher adult-child ratios, use of planned activities, and less perceived stress by caregivers. Implications of these findings are discussed.  相似文献   
222.
Seventy-two first, second and third grade boys and girls were administered seriation problems involving different combinations of shape and color variation. Success rates and latency scores were influenced by shape variation, but not by color variation or grade level. The failure to obtain predicted interactions between the effects of grade level and stimulus variation was attributed to the clarity of the relevant stimulus dimension.  相似文献   
223.
224.
5 blindfolded Ss were required to make absolute judgments of the extent to which their extended right arm was voluntarily moved in the horizontal plane. The first experiment entailed the judgment of 20 different amplitudes and from these data a scale of equal discriminability was constructed for each S. From these individual scales amplitudes were selected for 5 additional absolute judgment experiments where the number of amplitudes were varied from 4 to 16. Analysis of the mean equal discriminability scale showed that kinesthetic sensitivity varied over the continuum of movements. The primary analysis of information transfer between number of amplitudes of movement and responses indicated that information transfer varied considerably over the 5 experiments with a maximum transfer of 2.48 bits occurring when 16 amplitudes were used. These results were discussed in terms of the possible cues involved in movement discrimination and whether kinesthetic cues could be used in a closed-loop model of voluntary movement control.  相似文献   
225.
A horizontal straight-arm movement was used to determine the relationship between the physical stimulus continuum and the psychological continuum of kinesthetic extent of arm movement. A power function with an exponent of 1.075 best described this relationship. In addition, to classify extent of movement as belonging to either a prothetic or metathetic continuum, the category scale was derived. According to the functional criteria described by Stevens (1957), extent of movement was subserved by a metathetic process which would indicate that, for the present task, judgments were made through the use of positional cues rather than amplitude cues.  相似文献   
226.
The retention characteristics of several cues thought to underlie movement reproduction ability were examined and the results were discussed in terms of two models of motor short-term memory (Laabs, 1973; Pepper & Herman, 1970). Trace decay was indexed by constant error and not variable error. It appeared that the movement cues studied all had access to the central processing capacity in that forgetting did not occur until rehearsal was blocked by the introduction of a secondary task. However, there was some evidence to indicate that different cues are centrally represented in varying degrees of exactness. In this respect reliance on active movement cues and location cues produced better reproduction than passive movement and distance cues, respectively. The existence of an adaptation level established from the range of movement utilized was supported, and short movements were more dependent on central processing capacity than were long movements.  相似文献   
227.
Five predicted relationships between age and intellectual level and 16 Rorschach variables were examined through a cross-sectional analysis of 47 healthy, community-dwelling elderly men and women. Subjects were compared by age and intellectual level using gender, level of medication, and scores on the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) and the Geriatric Social Readjustment Questionnaire (GSRQ) as covariates. Screening tests for psychiatric and physical illness, recent life events, and hearing and vision loss were administered, In contrast to prior research suggesting changes in Rorschach scores with differences in age and intellectual level, only one significant difference was found for these variables. The data suggest that age and intellectual level may contribute less to Rorschach responses than was previously thought.  相似文献   
228.
Three clinical psychologists were asked to rate the MAPS figures in terms of how much social withdrawal from people the usage of the type of figure would represent if it were used on the test. Their mean ratings were compared with the actual MAPS test performances of normal and schizophrenic patients. The data indicated that, although, psychologists could reliably rate the amount of social withdrawal associated with the figure types, there was no difference in social withdrawal scores between normals and schizophrenics.  相似文献   
229.
In 2 studies, we examined the reliability and validity of an interpersonal measure of schizoid personality disorder (SZPD) based on nonverbal behaviors and interpersonal interactions occurring during interviews. A total of 556 male jail inmates in the United States participated in Study 1; 175 mentally disordered offenders in maximum security hospitals in the United Kingdom participated in Study 2. Across both samples, scores on the Interpersonal Measure of Schizoid Personality Disorder (IM–SZ) exhibited adequate reliability and patterns of correlations with other measures consistent with expectations. The scale displayed patterns of relatively specific correlations with interview and self-report measures of SZPD. In addition, the IM–SZ correlated in an expected manner with features of psychopathy and antisocial personality and with independent ratings of interpersonal behavior. We address implications for assessment of personality disorder.  相似文献   
230.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号