全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1375篇 |
免费 | 40篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
1416篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 9篇 |
2020年 | 15篇 |
2019年 | 19篇 |
2018年 | 19篇 |
2017年 | 29篇 |
2016年 | 30篇 |
2015年 | 13篇 |
2014年 | 31篇 |
2013年 | 129篇 |
2012年 | 42篇 |
2011年 | 54篇 |
2010年 | 31篇 |
2009年 | 32篇 |
2008年 | 58篇 |
2007年 | 45篇 |
2006年 | 41篇 |
2005年 | 34篇 |
2004年 | 45篇 |
2003年 | 48篇 |
2002年 | 38篇 |
2001年 | 19篇 |
2000年 | 27篇 |
1999年 | 25篇 |
1998年 | 21篇 |
1997年 | 22篇 |
1996年 | 25篇 |
1995年 | 23篇 |
1994年 | 24篇 |
1993年 | 19篇 |
1992年 | 25篇 |
1991年 | 13篇 |
1990年 | 20篇 |
1989年 | 12篇 |
1988年 | 12篇 |
1987年 | 15篇 |
1986年 | 18篇 |
1985年 | 21篇 |
1984年 | 30篇 |
1983年 | 30篇 |
1982年 | 11篇 |
1981年 | 22篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 26篇 |
1976年 | 15篇 |
1975年 | 26篇 |
1974年 | 13篇 |
1973年 | 17篇 |
1972年 | 14篇 |
排序方式: 共有1416条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
241.
Developing survey measures of religion is deceptively difficult. As religion in America becomes more diverse and research projects become more global, finding a shared vocabulary that taps into concepts of interest and selecting response categories that capture the full range of choices becomes ever more challenging. This research project introduces new tools and techniques for exploring and evaluating past survey measures of religion, including a new software tool that draws on the ARDA's massive collection of survey measures. We offer two examples to demonstrate how these resources can serve to evaluate past survey measures of religion and improve future measures. 相似文献
242.
David W. Johnson Roger T. Johnson Cary Roseth Tae Seob Shin 《Journal of applied social psychology》2014,44(9):622-633
This meta‐analysis investigates the degree to which achievement is positively associated with motivation within situations characterized by positive, negative, and no interdependence. First, the relative effects of positive, negative, and no interdependence on motivation and achievement were determined. Then the amount of variance in achievement explained by motivation (and vice versa) was calculated. In all, 629 independent studies were included, representing 26 different countries. Results also showed that motivation accounted for 14% of the variance in achievement (and vice versa). When the lowest‐quality studies were eliminated, the percentage of achievement explained by motivation increased to 24%. Positive interdependence resulted in greater motivation and achievement than did negative or no interdependence. Implications for theory and application are discussed. 相似文献
243.
A taxonomy of part-whole or meronymic relations is developed to explain the ordinary English-speaker's use of the term “part of” and its cognates. The resulting classification yields six types of meronymic relations: 1. component-integral object (pedal-bike), 2. member-collection (ship-fleet), 3. portion-mass (slice-pie), 4. stuff-object (steel-car), 5. feature-activity (paying-shopping), and 6. place-area (Everglades-Florida). Meronymic relations ore further distinguished from other inclusion relations, such as spatial inclusion, and class inclusion, and from several other semantic relations: attribution, attachment, and ownership. This taxonomy is then used to explain cases of apparent intransitivity in merological syllogisms, and standard form syllogisms whose premises express different inclusion relations. The data suggest that intransitivities arise due to equivocations between different types of semantic relations. These results are then explained by means of the relation element theory which accounts for the character and behavior of semantic relations in terms of more primitive relational elements. The inferential phenomena observed are then explained by means of a single principle of element matching. 相似文献
244.
245.
This investigation extends previous research documenting differences in Chinese and European American infants' facial expressivity. Chinese girls adopted by European American families, nonadopted Mainland Chinese girls, nonadopted Chinese American girls, and nonadopted European American girls responded to emotionally evocative slides and an odor stimulus. European American girls smiled more than Mainland Chinese and Chinese American girls and scored higher than Mainland Chinese girls for disgust-related expressions and overall expressivity. Adopted Chinese girls produced more disgust-related expressions than Mainland Chinese girls. Self-reported maternal strictness, aggravation, positive expressiveness, and cultural identification correlated with children's facial responses, as did number of siblings and adults in the home. Results suggest that culture and family environment influences facial expressivity, creating differences among children of the same ethnicity. 相似文献
246.
Nosofsky RM Stanton RD 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2006,32(2):314-334
Observers made speeded old-new recognition judgments of color stimuli embedded in a multidimensional similarity space. The paradigm used multiple lists but with the underlying similarity structures repeated across lists, to allow for quantitative modeling of the data at the individual-participant and individual-item levels. Correct-rejection response times (RTs) got systematically faster as the similarity of foils to the old study items decreased. There were also intricate patterns of speed-accuracy trade-offs that varied across individual items and participants. An exemplar-based random-walk model provided a good overall quantitative account of the recognition choice probabilities, mean correct RTs, and mean error RTs associated with the individual items on the basis of their positions in multidimensional similarity space. However, the model failed to predict the very long RTs associated with correct rejections of a prototype foil. 相似文献
247.
248.
Roger O. Gervais Dustin B. Wygant Martin Sellbom Yossef S. Ben-Porath 《Journal of personality assessment》2013,95(5):508-517
Existing measures of approval motivation attempt to assess this dimension of personality indirectly through evaluation of socially desirable response tendencies. The Martin-Larsen Approval Motivation (MLAM) Scale takes a more direct approach by focusing on individual differences in need for social approval. Since the original version of the MLAM scale is subject to acquiescence response bias, fully balanced and partially balanced scales were created to minimize this response style. Results indicated that the revised measures arc equivalent to the original version and that balancing affects the magnitude but not the pattern of correlates with other scales. Comparisons with the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability (MCSD) Scale revealed a divergent pattern of correlates with several measures of personality suggesting a basic difference between the MLAM and MCSD in their conceptualization of approval motivation. 相似文献
249.
Scott E. Culhane Osvaldo F. Morera P. J. Watson Roger E. Millsap 《Journal of personality assessment》2013,95(4):387-395
We collected data from a predominately Anglo American student sample in the Southeastern United States and a predominately Hispanic student sample in the Southwestern United States. Along with an assessment of internal consistency reliability, we examined measurement invariance of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale–20 (TAS–20) using confirmatory factor analysis. We also assessed the predictive invariance of the TAS–20. Results indicate that 2 of the 3 TAS–20 subscales demonstrated satisfactory internal consistency reliability across samples. Items from the TAS–20 subscales demonstrated measurement invariance of the latent means. The relationship between 2 measures of emotional dysfunction and the TAS–20 also demonstrated slope and intercept invariance, indicating equivalent validity. 相似文献
250.
Roger Schmit 《逻辑史和逻辑哲学》2013,34(1):5-17
Die Zusammenhänge die zwischen G. Freges und R. H. Lotzes logischen Lehren bestehen, sind, wie die gemeinsame Beurteilung der Gebrauchssprache zeigt, noch tiefer als allgemein angenommen. Insbesondere die von Frege konzipierte logische Sprachkritik ist in drei Punkten von Lotze beeinflußt. Lotze fordert nämlich die strenge Trennung von Logik und Gebrauchssprache. Daneben spielt der Begriff des Logischeinfachen eine zentrale Rolle in seiner Logik. Schließlich unterscheidet er den objektiven Gedanken von seiner Färbung. The connexions that exist between the logical doctrines of G. Frege and R. H. Lotze are, as shows their common treatment of natural language, deeper than is generally admitted. In particular, the logical criticism of language conceived by Frege is influenced in three points by Lotze. Firstly, Lotze postulates the strict separation of logic and natural language. Furthermore, the idea of logical simplicity plays an important role in his logic. Finally, he distinguishes objective thought from its tone. 相似文献