全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1276篇 |
免费 | 39篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
1316篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 8篇 |
2020年 | 11篇 |
2019年 | 18篇 |
2018年 | 14篇 |
2017年 | 22篇 |
2016年 | 28篇 |
2015年 | 11篇 |
2014年 | 26篇 |
2013年 | 119篇 |
2012年 | 41篇 |
2011年 | 50篇 |
2010年 | 30篇 |
2009年 | 27篇 |
2008年 | 55篇 |
2007年 | 42篇 |
2006年 | 36篇 |
2005年 | 32篇 |
2004年 | 44篇 |
2003年 | 46篇 |
2002年 | 37篇 |
2001年 | 17篇 |
2000年 | 26篇 |
1999年 | 22篇 |
1998年 | 20篇 |
1997年 | 22篇 |
1996年 | 24篇 |
1995年 | 21篇 |
1994年 | 23篇 |
1993年 | 19篇 |
1992年 | 24篇 |
1991年 | 12篇 |
1990年 | 17篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1988年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 15篇 |
1986年 | 17篇 |
1985年 | 17篇 |
1984年 | 29篇 |
1983年 | 29篇 |
1982年 | 11篇 |
1981年 | 22篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 26篇 |
1976年 | 15篇 |
1975年 | 25篇 |
1974年 | 12篇 |
1973年 | 16篇 |
1972年 | 12篇 |
排序方式: 共有1316条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
151.
Roger Hooker 《Islam & Christian-Muslim Relations》1995,6(2):211-228
In 1947 India became independent and at the same time lost a large part of its territory to the newly created state of Pakistan. This new political arrangement was achieved at a terrible cost in terms of human life and suffering. The years up to and immediately following 1947 provoked a debate about national and human identity. Pakistan was founded on the grounds that Muslims could not be safe or prosperous under majority Hindu rule in newly independent India. The new India insisted that she was a secular democracy in which all religious communities could enjoy equal status. The debate is reflected in a number of Hindi‐language novels which deal with this period. The present article is about one of them, written by a member of India's small Shi'ite Muslim community who was also a Marxist. The debate raises issues about human identity which we can now see as being tragically and importantly relevant for contemporary Europe. These issues are not only political but also religious in that they go to the heart of our understanding of what it means to be human beings. The article points to some of these issues and at the same time argues that the novel as a literary form provides a valuable and significant vehicle for their discussion. 相似文献
152.
The authors used 6-year longitudinal data from the Victoria Longitudinal Study (VLS) to investigate individual differences in amount of episodic memory change. Latent change models revealed reliable individual differences in cognitive change. Changes in episodic memory were significantly correlated with changes in other cognitive variables, including speed and working memory. A structural equation model for the latent change scores showed that changes in speed and working memory predicted changes in episodic memory, as expected by processing resource theory. However, these effects were best modeled as being mediated by changes in induction and fact retrieval. Dissociations were detected between cross-sectional ability correlations and longitudinal changes. Shuffling the tasks used to define the Working Memory latent variable altered patterns of change correlations. 相似文献
153.
Roger Wales Michelle Colman Philippa Pattison 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1983,36(1):1-17
Mothers' selections of category names in object-naming tasks were examined using the levels of categorization defined by Eleanor Rosch (E. Rosch. In T. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language, New York: Academic Press, 1973. E. Rosch. In E. Rosch & B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1978. E. Rosch, C. Mervis, W. Gray, D. Johnson, & P. Boyes-Braem. Cognitive Psychology, 1976, 8, 382–439). The effect of three variables—Presentation Context, Taxonomy, and Age (of person for whom mother was naming the items)—on mothers' choice of category names, was also investigated. The stimuli were 36 photographs of common concrete objects representative of four taxonomies. Forty mothers named the stimulus items for their 2-year-olds (N = 20), for their 4-year-olds (N = 20), and for the Experimenter. The category labels that children of both ages used when naming the same items, and mothers' independent predictions of the names their children would use in the naming task, were recorded. Contextual factors were found to have a primary role in determining mothers' selections of category names and the three independent variables had a complex and dynamic effect on mothers' naming practices. 相似文献
154.
In three experiments, rats learned to respond differentially to reinforced and nonreinforced trials when the length of the intertrial interval (ITI) predicted the trial outcome (reinforcement or nonreinforcement). Rats in control groups, for whom the length of the ITI did not predict the outcome, did not show differential performance on nonreinforced and reinforced trials. Generalization gradients obtained following discrimination training were comparable to those obtained following discrimination training with other types of discriminative stimuli. That is, groups which had shown differential performance in discrimination training yielded generalization gradients with fastest speeds at the previously reinforced ITIs, slowest speeds at the previously nonreinforced ITIs, and intermediate speeds at ITIs of intermediate length. Control groups yielded flat gradients across all ITIs tested. These effects were shown for relative time discrimination (short time = reinforcement, long time = nonreinforcement, or the reverse) and also for an absolute time discrimination (long and short time = reinforcement, middle time = nonreinforcement or the reverse). This method was effective for time duration measured in minutes rather than seconds, as is more commonly the case. 相似文献
155.
Eliot Hearst Minnie B. Koresko Roger Poppen 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1964,7(5):369-380
Generalization gradients along a line-tilt continuum were obtained from groups of pigeons that had been trained to peck a key on different schedules of reinforcement. In Exp. I, gradients following training on a differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate (DRL) schedule proved to be much flatter than gradients following the usual 1-min variable interval (VI) training. In Exp. II, the value of the VI schedule itself was parametrically studied; Ss trained on long VI schedules (e.g., 4-min) produced much flatter gradients than Ss trained on short VI schedules (30-sec; 1-min). The results were interpreted mainly in terms of the relative control exerted by internal, proprioceptive cues on the different reinforcement schedules. Several implications of the results for other problems in the field of stimulus generalization are discussed. 相似文献
156.
157.
Mary Gallagher Rob Millar Owen Hargie Roger Ellis 《British Journal of Guidance & Counselling》1992,20(3):274-290
The aim of this study was to ascertain the self-perceived social and personal concerns of adolescents attending educational institutions in Northern Ireland. 446 questionnaires from 15-18-year-old students at ten institutions were analysed in order to determine the frequency with which respondents reported worrying about the following broad areas: school, choosing a job, finding a job, home, starting work, opposite-sex relationships, personal issues, and relating to others. The effects of variables such as gender, age and nature of educational institution attended were also examined. The results indicated that most frequent worry was reported about finding and choosing employment, personal issues and opposite-sex relationships. Gender, age and religious affiliation of school attended exerted major effects. Implications for those involved in working with adolescents within educational contexts are discussed. 相似文献
158.
Arthur J. Reynolds Roger P. Weissberg Wesley J. Kasprow 《American journal of community psychology》1992,20(5):599-624
Investigated predictors of five measures of early school adjustment for an ethnically diverse cohort of 683 inner-city kindergartners and first graders. Data from 2 consecutive years were collected from teachers, school records, and children. A multiple-regression preduction model significantly explained children's competence behavior, problem behavior, reading achievement, mathematics achievement, and school absences. Prior adjustment and sociodemographic factors explained a majority of the variance in adjustment. Perceived quality of parent involvement was signifcantly related (in the expected direction) to all five outcomes. Exposure to life events was significantly associated in the expected direction with competence behavior, problem behavior, and school absences but not with reading and mathematics achievement. Together, parent involvement and life-event variables explained as much as 12% of the variance in adjustment independent of sociodemographic and prior adjustment factors. The role of family and school factors in the adjustment of children at risk is discussed [corrected]. 相似文献
159.
Roger Schmit 《Husserl Studies》1992,9(1):31-49
Ohne ZusammenfassungZu danken habe ich den Herren Ernst Wolfgang Orth und Riccardo Pozzo für die freundliche Durchsicht des Manuskripts und wertvolle Hinweise. 相似文献
160.
A study of six Broca's aphasics is reported. The task was to complete sentence frames with a missing word whose type was systematically varied by syntactic form class (nouns, verbs, prepositions, and particles) and the number of lexical options appropriate to any given instance. The primary aim was to contrast syntactic type (content vs. function words) and level of sentential constraint as ways of determining whether there was a central syntactic component to the language deficit. There was a significant effect for word type and an interaction between word type and level of constraint. These results are discussed as supporting a syntactic interpretation of the deficit. 相似文献