全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1309篇 |
免费 | 40篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
1350篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 8篇 |
2020年 | 12篇 |
2019年 | 20篇 |
2018年 | 15篇 |
2017年 | 23篇 |
2016年 | 30篇 |
2015年 | 12篇 |
2014年 | 26篇 |
2013年 | 124篇 |
2012年 | 44篇 |
2011年 | 54篇 |
2010年 | 32篇 |
2009年 | 28篇 |
2008年 | 56篇 |
2007年 | 43篇 |
2006年 | 37篇 |
2005年 | 33篇 |
2004年 | 44篇 |
2003年 | 48篇 |
2002年 | 37篇 |
2001年 | 18篇 |
2000年 | 26篇 |
1999年 | 22篇 |
1998年 | 20篇 |
1997年 | 22篇 |
1996年 | 25篇 |
1995年 | 21篇 |
1994年 | 23篇 |
1993年 | 19篇 |
1992年 | 24篇 |
1991年 | 13篇 |
1990年 | 17篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1988年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 16篇 |
1986年 | 17篇 |
1985年 | 18篇 |
1984年 | 29篇 |
1983年 | 29篇 |
1982年 | 11篇 |
1981年 | 22篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 26篇 |
1976年 | 15篇 |
1975年 | 25篇 |
1974年 | 12篇 |
1973年 | 16篇 |
1972年 | 12篇 |
排序方式: 共有1350条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
131.
Janusz Czelakowski Roger Maddux Gerhard Schurz Kazimierz Trzesicki 《Studia Logica》1986,45(2):223-229
132.
Geoffrey L. Thorpe Ph.D. Roger B. Frey Ph.D. 《Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy》1996,14(3):193-198
Previous studies have shown that the Common Beliefs Survey III (CBS III) is an irrational beliefs inventory with satisfactory psychometric properties. We extended research on the CBS by examining this 54-item inventory for redundant material, and substantially abbreviated it without compormising its reliability or validity. This article describes the development of the new, 19-item short form (CBS19), convenient for clinical use, that preserves the psychometric properties of the original. 相似文献
133.
Average time required to determine whether an alphanumeric character was presented in its normal version or as its mirror image increased from 500 msec to 1,000 msec as its angular departure from upright increased from 0 to 180 deg. However, when Ss already knew the identity of the upcoming character and when advance information as to its orientation was available for 1,000 msec, this reaction time was reduced to about 400 msec regardless of the orientation of the test stimulus. In this case, Ss claimed that they could prepare for the rotated stimulus by imagining the normal version of the designated character rotated into the indicated orientation and that they could then rapidly test for a match against the ensuing stimulus. 相似文献
134.
An inexpensive tachistoscope for student labs is described that utilizes a camera shutter to control exposure duration and a potentiometer to vary light intensity. Stimuli can be drawn or typed on 3 x 5 index cards. 相似文献
135.
Roger L. Barrett 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1968,47(4):353-357
Subjects in this study were 100 ninth-grade students who expressed an interest in receiving test results. Subjects were assigned to one of four groups on the basis of self-regard measures. Treatment consisted of reporting standardized test results to the experimental groups but not to the control groups. Immediately following test reporting and two weeks afterward, students characterized by high self-regard made significantly more accurate self-estimates of interests, aptitudes, and achievements than did low self-regard students. In addition, accuracy of self-estimates was significantly greater for students receiving reports of test results than for students not receiving test results. 相似文献
136.
Roger W. Russell Robert G. F. Pretty 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1951,3(4):151-156
Predictions from Maier's theory of “frustration”-instigated behaviour have been tested in an experimental situation differing significantly from that in which the theory was propounded yet containing the central element of “frustration”—the insoluble problem.
A water discrimination unit was employed in which the performance of rats would be observed during attacks on insoluble problems, position problems or symbol problems.
Two groups, each containing ten Wistar albino rats, served as subjects. The research design consisted of the following phases: preliminary training, development of position responses, exposure to a symbol-reward problem with 50 per cent, punishment and exposure to a symbol-reward problem with 100 per cent, punishment. The design differed for the two groups only at the phase in which the position responses were established. During this phase one group was exposed to a position-reward problem and the other to an insoluble problem.
Position responses were established as frequently under position-“frustration” (position stereotypes) as under position-reward (position habits) conditions. Position stereotypes were more rigid—more resistant to extinction—than position habits under conditions of 50 per cent, punishment. Position stereotypes were as readily extinguished under 100 per cent, punishment as were position habits under 30 per cent, punishment.
The first two observations conform to predictions made from Maier's theory. The third does not. That is to say, not all situations containing the basic elements of “frustration” give rise to stereotyped behaviour patterns which are as rigid or “fixated” as Maier's theory would predict. It is a reasonable hypothesis that the characteristics of stereotyped responses established in certain “frustration” situations may be described adequately in terms of conventional learning principles without the necessity of resorting to a distinction between “goal-motivated” and “frustration-instigated” behaviour. 相似文献
A water discrimination unit was employed in which the performance of rats would be observed during attacks on insoluble problems, position problems or symbol problems.
Two groups, each containing ten Wistar albino rats, served as subjects. The research design consisted of the following phases: preliminary training, development of position responses, exposure to a symbol-reward problem with 50 per cent, punishment and exposure to a symbol-reward problem with 100 per cent, punishment. The design differed for the two groups only at the phase in which the position responses were established. During this phase one group was exposed to a position-reward problem and the other to an insoluble problem.
Position responses were established as frequently under position-“frustration” (position stereotypes) as under position-reward (position habits) conditions. Position stereotypes were more rigid—more resistant to extinction—than position habits under conditions of 50 per cent, punishment. Position stereotypes were as readily extinguished under 100 per cent, punishment as were position habits under 30 per cent, punishment.
The first two observations conform to predictions made from Maier's theory. The third does not. That is to say, not all situations containing the basic elements of “frustration” give rise to stereotyped behaviour patterns which are as rigid or “fixated” as Maier's theory would predict. It is a reasonable hypothesis that the characteristics of stereotyped responses established in certain “frustration” situations may be described adequately in terms of conventional learning principles without the necessity of resorting to a distinction between “goal-motivated” and “frustration-instigated” behaviour. 相似文献
137.
Roger Ulrich Marshall Wolfe Sylvia Dulaney 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1969,12(6):1009-1015
Squirrel monkeys were restrained in a chair equipped with a tail-shock apparatus and a pneumatic bite hose located in front of the subject's face. An aggressive response was recorded when the monkey bit the hose. Initial sessions in which no shocks were delivered produced some biting. When biting during these sessions stabilized at a near-zero level, regularly scheduled shocks were delivered to the monkey's tail, causing a consistently higher rate of biting. After several sessions under these conditions, a punishment phase was introduced in which the previous shock conditions were maintained, and every bite was followed immediately by another, more intense shock. Biting under these conditions was suppressed to a near-zero level. When the punishment contingency was removed, biting increased. With one subject, two additional bite-contingent stimuli were examined: (1) a milder shock that, when made contingent upon hose biting, also suppressed that response, and (2) a contingent tone that had no obvious suppressing or facilitating effect. Individual differences among subjects were extreme, but the effect of bite-contingent shock was consistent. Observations of the subjects during the punishment sessions indicated the existence of certain side effects that resulted from the use of punishment to suppress shock-induced aggression. 相似文献
138.
Roger A. Shiner 《The Southern journal of philosophy》1975,13(3):383-392
139.
140.
Roger J. Quy Edward W. Kubiak 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1974,26(4):561-565
“Aware” (A) and “naive” (N) groups received different instructional sets, the former being informed both of the nature of their task and the response-reinforcement contingency. Negative reinforcement was given to these groups whenever a spontaneous GSR was emitted during four 4-min trial periods. Two corresponding yoked-control groups, CA and CN, received non-contingent reinforcement over the same periods. The contingent reinforcement groups both showed learned suppression of spontaneous GSR activity, but comparison between the A and N groups revealed a significant interaction between Time and Instructional Set. It is suggested that the “aware” instructional set had an inhibitory effect upon learning. 相似文献