首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   5244篇
  免费   218篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2023年   47篇
  2022年   71篇
  2021年   107篇
  2020年   119篇
  2019年   152篇
  2018年   219篇
  2017年   206篇
  2016年   181篇
  2015年   139篇
  2014年   144篇
  2013年   620篇
  2012年   309篇
  2011年   322篇
  2010年   223篇
  2009年   152篇
  2008年   285篇
  2007年   249篇
  2006年   218篇
  2005年   178篇
  2004年   163篇
  2003年   136篇
  2002年   124篇
  2001年   69篇
  2000年   75篇
  1999年   61篇
  1998年   48篇
  1997年   43篇
  1996年   39篇
  1995年   42篇
  1994年   43篇
  1993年   41篇
  1992年   39篇
  1991年   28篇
  1990年   44篇
  1989年   44篇
  1988年   39篇
  1987年   31篇
  1986年   25篇
  1985年   28篇
  1984年   30篇
  1983年   24篇
  1982年   33篇
  1981年   31篇
  1980年   23篇
  1978年   24篇
  1976年   21篇
  1975年   20篇
  1973年   20篇
  1971年   16篇
  1968年   15篇
排序方式: 共有5464条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
131.
A group of young children (mean age: 2.5 yr) were instructed to follow different requests by a teacher in a day-care setting. Experiment I verified that mean group instruction following was low (10%) despite the opportunity for “observational learning”, i.e., the group of 12 children could watch a nonreinforced adult comply with the teacher's request. In Experiment II, when positive consequences were provided contingent on the adult model's behavior, mean group instruction following was relatively unaffected (14%). When direct reinforcement was given to four peer models, each for several sessions, the individual performances of three of the four peer models was elevated (from 50% to 80%); however, the mean performance of the remaining nonreinforced children (N = 7) was only moderately affected (21%). When reinforcement contingencies were again changed, so that each group member was provided direct, but intermittent reinforcement, mean group performance increased substantially to levels of over 70%. Once instruction following was high, presentation of reinforcement only to one peer model sufficed to maintain performance whereas earlier, this same vicarious reinforcement procedure had failed to establish group compliance. The maintenance of instruction-following behavior when reinforcement was applied solely to one child was interpreted mainly in terms of a high resistance to extinction following a history of intermittent reinforcement rather than a “vicarious”- or “self”-reinforcement mechanism. Finally, removal and re-introduction of group intermittent reinforcement, respectively, lowered performance (to levels of 40%) and elevated (to levels of 65%) the group's performance.  相似文献   
132.
133.
The proposition that impulsive children inhibit expressive behavior less than reflective children was investigated by providing third grade impulsive and reflective boys with either differential or nondifferential forms or successive discrimination training prior to tests for stimulus generalization. Consistent with specific predictions derived from Spence's theory on the roles of excitation and inhibition, it was found that: (a) Reflectives acquired the discrimination more rapidly, (b) reflectives achieved a higher level of discriminative performance, (c) impulsives exhibited more variable response reates during the nonreinforced stimulus, and (d) the type of training interacted with the child's conceptual tempo to determine the elevation and shape of the post-discrimination generalization gradients. The results provide support for the construct validity of the Matching Familiar Figures Test of reflectivity-impulsivity.  相似文献   
134.
This paper selectively reviews an area of operations research, refered to as normative location theory, that deals with the placement of objects in continuous space. The computational procedures discussed in this literature require initially that a certain set objects be located a priori; the placement of the objects in a second set is then determined in such a way that the total “interaction” among the fixed and variable objects is minimized. A number of strategies appropriate for different distance functions are surveyed and a numerical example is given illustrating one particular iterative algorithm. Although the current versions of these location methods depend upon more than the nonmetric information contained in the proximity measures defined for all pairs of objects, they can be generalized, and consequently, several possible connections to nonmetric multidimensional scaling are pointed out.  相似文献   
135.
A TASTE FOR HUME     
Patricia de  Martelaere 《Ratio》1989,2(2):122-137
  相似文献   
136.
137.
138.
139.
140.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号