首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   8123篇
  免费   458篇
  国内免费   6篇
  8587篇
  2024年   17篇
  2023年   104篇
  2022年   114篇
  2021年   153篇
  2020年   229篇
  2019年   222篇
  2018年   405篇
  2017年   396篇
  2016年   367篇
  2015年   238篇
  2014年   280篇
  2013年   1035篇
  2012年   591篇
  2011年   604篇
  2010年   364篇
  2009年   237篇
  2008年   504篇
  2007年   387篇
  2006年   368篇
  2005年   281篇
  2004年   211篇
  2003年   186篇
  2002年   164篇
  2001年   92篇
  2000年   121篇
  1999年   80篇
  1998年   47篇
  1997年   46篇
  1996年   38篇
  1995年   30篇
  1994年   22篇
  1993年   31篇
  1992年   49篇
  1991年   41篇
  1990年   41篇
  1989年   38篇
  1988年   34篇
  1987年   26篇
  1986年   18篇
  1985年   32篇
  1984年   29篇
  1983年   24篇
  1979年   17篇
  1978年   24篇
  1976年   18篇
  1974年   25篇
  1973年   18篇
  1969年   15篇
  1968年   17篇
  1966年   15篇
排序方式: 共有8587条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
In five experiments, we examined 3‐ to 6‐year‐olds’ understanding that they could gain knowledge indirectly from someone who had seen something they had not. Consistent with previous research, children judged that an informant, who had seen inside a box, knew its contents. Similarly, when an informant marked a picture to indicate her suggestion as to the content of the box, 3‐ to 4‐year‐olds trusted this more frequently when the informant had seen inside the box than when she had not. Going beyond previous research, 3‐ to 4‐year‐olds were also sensitive to informants’ relevant experience when they had to look over a barrier to see the marked picture, or ask for the barrier to be raised. Yet when children had to elicit the informant's suggestion, rather than just consult a suggestion already present, even 4‐ to 5‐year‐olds were no more likely to do so when the informant had seen the box's content than when she had not, and no more likely to trust the well‐informed suggestion than the uninformed one. We conclude that young children who can ask questions may not yet fully understand the process by which they can gain accurate information from someone who has the experience they lack.  相似文献   
4.
A growing literature has observed a significant reduction in pain sensitivity among hypertensive animals and humans. It is uncertain whether a reduced sensitivity to pain can be observed in nonnotensive individuals who go on to develop high blood pressure. Blood pressure (BP) was reassessed in one hundred fifteen 19-year-old boys initially tested at age 14, when they were also presented with a pain stimulus (mechanical finger pressure). Hierarchical regression analyses indicated that information regarding pain tolerance improved prediction of changes in systolic and diastolic blood pressure beyond that afforded by differences in BP at age 14, parental history of hypertension, and body mass index. These analyses suggest that pain sensitivity may be associated with physiological processes involved in the development of sustained high blood pressure.  相似文献   
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
The processing of restrictive relative clauses in Hungarian   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
B MacWhinney  C Pléh 《Cognition》1988,29(2):95-141
  相似文献   
10.
Fifty 8-week-old Balb/c mice were individually identified and housed together in a large and enriched environment for 5 months. Maze and open field exploration, response to an aversive noise, swimming, and induced grooming tests were applied to each mouse in an initial search for possible relationships between brain morphology and spontaneous behavior in isogenic individuals living in a complex social and physical environment. The tasks generated 39 quantitative behavioral indices which include locomotion, rearing, still, and grooming bout frequencies, latencies, total, and mean bout durations. At the end of the tests, the 7-month-old mice were sacrificed and the fresh weights of their whole brain, cerebellum, brain stem, diencephalon, telencephalon, and prosencephalon were rapidly obtained. Behavioral data have wide variations and do not adjust to normal population distributions. Means of the same parameter differ between tests. A Spearman correlation matrix of all data yielded many significant correlations between indices of the same task which can be interpreted in terms of time budget and sequence probability. Significant correlations between indices of different tests suggest diverse emotionalities, exploratory strategies, and motor skills. The correlations between body and brain weights and among separate brain regions were not significant. There were several low but significant correlations between brain weights and behavioral indices. Such correlations, the resulting factors, and significant behavioral differences between mice with large and small brains suggest that mice displaying low motor activity in novel environments have larger brains and forebrain/hindbrain ratios than mice with high activity, and that animals with high scores of some specific behaviors have larger brain areas physiologically related to such behaviors.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号