全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1162篇 |
免费 | 64篇 |
专业分类
1226篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 8篇 |
2023年 | 6篇 |
2022年 | 11篇 |
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 22篇 |
2019年 | 20篇 |
2018年 | 31篇 |
2017年 | 33篇 |
2016年 | 74篇 |
2015年 | 32篇 |
2014年 | 46篇 |
2013年 | 130篇 |
2012年 | 59篇 |
2011年 | 66篇 |
2010年 | 43篇 |
2009年 | 33篇 |
2008年 | 64篇 |
2007年 | 55篇 |
2006年 | 40篇 |
2005年 | 58篇 |
2004年 | 34篇 |
2003年 | 33篇 |
2002年 | 34篇 |
2001年 | 14篇 |
2000年 | 22篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 20篇 |
1997年 | 18篇 |
1996年 | 9篇 |
1995年 | 14篇 |
1994年 | 18篇 |
1993年 | 12篇 |
1992年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 12篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 13篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1983年 | 13篇 |
1982年 | 8篇 |
1981年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 10篇 |
1979年 | 11篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有1226条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
311.
312.
Ehringer MA Rhee SH Young S Corley R Hewitt JK 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2006,34(1):1-17
We report findings based on analyses of self-reports of six common adolescent psychopathologies (attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, ADHD; conduct disorder, CD; oppositional defiant disorder, ODD; generalized anxiety disorder, GAD; separation anxiety disorder, SAD; and major depressive disorder, MDD) in a sample of 1,162 male and female adolescent (12–19 years) twin pairs and 426 siblings. Prevalence statistics for past year and lifetime reports confirm differences between genders for CD, GAD, SAD, and MDD, and a lack of differences between twins and their non-twin siblings. Biometrical modeling was conducted to ascertain the relative influences of genes, and shared and non-shared environments contributing to these disorders. A more robust estimate of these parameters was obtained by including non-twin siblings. Age-specific thresholds were integrated into the analyses to appropriately model the developmental patterns of behavior. We found evidence for both genetic and non-shared environmental influences for all disorders. Shared environmental influences also seem to be important for MDD and lifetime GAD. 相似文献
313.
Robin Usher 《Studies in Philosophy and Education》2006,25(4):279-288
Starting with Lyotard’s characterisation of postmodernity as incredulity, this is related to another of his key concepts—that of ‘performativity’. Lyotard appears to deploy performativity to characterise those technologies that bring about the optimisation of efficient performance. However, there is another sense of performativity where it is linked to performance. Performance conditions the possibility of any and all performatives, or to put it another way, as performance is itself enabled by performativity, so too performativity is realised through its performance. Both senses of performativity and the linkage between them are clearly manifested in the space of knowledge production that is the contemporary university. This linkage is itself a feature of the semiotic process within which contemporary knowledge and knowledge production is located and which Lyotard himself recognised, albeit implicitly. The implication of this therefore is that Lyotard himself performs his text and in so doing both manifests and contributes to realising both senses of performativity. 相似文献
314.
Anticipation skill and susceptibility to deceptive movement 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
The ability to detect deceptive movement was examined in skilled and novice rugby players. Participants (14 per group) attempted to predict direction change from video of expert and recreational rugby players changing direction with and without deceptive movement. Confidence associated with judgments was recorded on each trial to seek evidence regarding use of inferential (heuristic-based) and direct-perceptual (invariant-based) judgments. Novices were found to be susceptible to deceptive movement whereas skilled participants were not; however, both skilled and novice participants were more confident on trials containing deceptive movement. The data suggest that the skill-level difference in sensitivity to advance visual information extends to deceptive information. The implications of this finding, and the importance of considering the underlying process of anticipation skill, are discussed. 相似文献
315.
Epistemic contextualists think that the extension of the expression ‘knows’ (and its cognates) depends on and varies with the context of utterance. In the last 15 years or so this view has faced intense criticism. This paper focuses on two sorts of objections. The first are what I call the ‘linguistic objections’, which purport to show that the best available linguistic evidence suggests that ‘knows’ is not context-sensitive. The second is what I call the ‘disagreement problem’, which concerns the behaviour of ‘knows’ in disagreement reports. These may not be the only objections to epistemic contextualism, but they are probably the most influential. I argue that the best current epistemic contextualist response to the linguistic objection is incomplete, and I show how it can be supplemented to deal with the full range of linguistic objections. I also develop a new solution to the disagreement problem. The upshot is that neither sort of objection gives us any reason to reject epistemic contextualism. This conclusion is, in a sense, negative—no new arguments for epistemic contextualism are advanced—but it’s a vital step towards rehabilitating the view. 相似文献
316.
Robin McKenna 《Australasian journal of philosophy》2015,93(4):782-798
The target of this paper is the ‘simple’ knowledge account of assertion, according to which assertion is constituted by a single epistemic rule of the form ‘One must: assert p only if one knows p’ (where p is a proposition). My aim is to argue that those who are attracted to a knowledge account of assertion should prefer what I call the ‘complex’ knowledge account, according to which assertion is constituted by a system of rules all of which are, taken together, constitutive of assertion. One of those rules—which, following John Searle, I call the ‘preparatory condition’—is of the form ‘One must: assert p only if one knows p.’ All else being equal, simple accounts are preferable to complex accounts. I argue in this paper that all else isn't equal. While the simple knowledge account provides an elegant explanation of certain data, it is hard to see how to integrate the simple knowledge account into a more general theory of illocutionary acts. Because the complex knowledge account avoids this objection while explaining the same data as the simple knowledge account does, I conclude that the complex knowledge account is superior to the simple knowledge account. 相似文献
317.
Kathi Marymee Cynthia R. Dolan Roberta A. Pagon Robin L. Bennett Sandra Coe Nancy L. Fisher 《Journal of genetic counseling》1998,7(2):133-165
We present a method for the development of consensus documents describing the components of genetic evaluation and genetic counseling for various diagnoses. These documents were developed to encourage consistency among genetic professionals in Washington State. Other possible uses of these documents are to provide information regarding genetic evaluations for health care practitioners and payers, and to assist in quality assurance and genetic training programs. A working group of six genetic professionals developed two templates for the critical elements of genetic evaluation and genetic counseling, for clinical (nonprenatal) and prenatal patients. The working group then completed prototype templates for several specific genetic disorders. The templates and prototypes were sent to interested genetic professionals and perinatologists who submitted a total of 76 draft critical elements (CE's) to the working group. At two statewide meetings, participating practitioners modified and unanimously approved the CE templates, then unanimously approved the 21 draft CEs that had been finalized in small group discussions. Approved CE's were distributed to genetic professionals and perinatologists within the state. 相似文献
318.
Robin Skynner 《Journal of Family Therapy》1998,20(2):207-210
My paper in the first issue of the Journal of Family Therapy outlined some ideas which have continued to form the basis of my practice of family therapy ever since. Using the psychoanalytic concept of 'counter-transference' I suggested that opening themselves to the emotional interaction of the family will usually lead to a reproduction of the essential basic family conflict within the therapists' own psyches. In this way the therapist takes on the role assumed by many 'family scapegoats', expressing some emotion 'for' the family which other members dare not articulate or even entertain. 相似文献
319.
320.
A prospective study of memory for child sexual abuse: new findings relevant to the repressed-memory controversy 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Goodman GS Ghetti S Quas JA Edelstein RS Alexander KW Redlich AD Cordon IM Jones DP 《Psychological science》2003,14(2):113-118
Previous research indicates that many adults (nearly 40%) fail to report their own documented child sexual abuse (CSA) when asked about their childhood experiences. These controversial results could reflect lack of consciously accessible recollection, thus bolstering claims that traumatic memories may be repressed. In the present study, 175 individuals with documented CSA histories were interviewed regarding their childhood trauma. Unlike in previous studies, the majority of participants (81%) in our study reported the documented abuse. Older age when the abuse ended, maternal support following disclosure of the abuse, and more severe abuse were associated with an increased likelihood of disclosure. Ethnicity and dissociation also played a role. Failure to report CSA should not necessarily be interpreted as evidence that the abuse is inaccessible to memory, although inaccessibility or forgetting cannot be ruled out in a subset of cases. 相似文献