首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   523篇
  免费   23篇
  2024年   1篇
  2023年   10篇
  2022年   4篇
  2021年   8篇
  2020年   13篇
  2019年   17篇
  2018年   21篇
  2017年   19篇
  2016年   23篇
  2015年   12篇
  2014年   15篇
  2013年   52篇
  2012年   32篇
  2011年   39篇
  2010年   15篇
  2009年   19篇
  2008年   21篇
  2007年   25篇
  2006年   27篇
  2005年   17篇
  2004年   11篇
  2003年   13篇
  2002年   15篇
  2001年   14篇
  2000年   15篇
  1999年   5篇
  1998年   7篇
  1997年   11篇
  1996年   9篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   7篇
  1993年   11篇
  1992年   5篇
  1991年   6篇
  1990年   1篇
  1989年   4篇
  1988年   1篇
  1987年   2篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   1篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   1篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   1篇
  1979年   1篇
  1976年   1篇
  1975年   1篇
排序方式: 共有546条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
441.
Summary Let be the first order theory of diagonalizable algebras. We define a bijection from the atomic formulas of (identities) to the open formulas of . is an algebraic analogous of ≒. We prove that , -1 preserve the validity.The algebraization of the theories which express TheorSee the precedent papers with the same subtitle.Allatum est die 21 Julii 1975  相似文献   
442.
443.
In the present study the effects of task requirements were investigated using a same-different paradigm with geometric configurations as stimuli. Two conditions were employed: in the first, the instructions required the subjects to focus upon all dimensions of the stimuli; in the second, instead, the instructions required the subjects to attend only to some of the dimensions. The results show that in the first condition the subjects, in order to reach their decision, used a template- matching strategy, while in the second condition an analytic strategy was employed.  相似文献   
444.
445.
446.
A method is described for computing the centre of mass (COM) from empirical estimates of the centre of pressure (COP) obtained by means of a force platform. The method is based on a biomechanical model of sway movements in quiet standing, according to which the horizontal acceleration of the COM is approximately proportional to the COM–COP difference. The equations are solved by approximating the solution with best fitting spline functions. The implications for movement control are discussed.  相似文献   
447.
Sex differences in cognitive abilities were determined using the norms from two standardizations of the Differential Aptitude Test (DAT) and the Primary Mental Abilities (PMA) conducted between 1979 and 1995 in Spain. The standardized sex differences (ds) were computed separately for the DAT and the PMA subscales. Males scored higher in the DAT subscales Verbal Reasoning, Numerical Ability, Spatial Relations and Mechanical Reasoning, as well as in the PMA subscales Numerical Ability and Mental Rotation. Females scored higher in Inductive Reasoning (PMA-R) in the 1979 and 1995 standardizations. Taken together, these data do not support the hypothesis that cognitive sex differences are disappearing: there are still some differences favoring females and still some differences favoring males.  相似文献   
448.
Subjects studied words in a classification task (to what categories does it belong?) or in a production task (producing associates to the word) and then completed one of two implicit memory tests—category association or free association. The classification study task and the category-association test emphasize categorical relations between concepts, and the production study task and the free-association test draw on associative relations. As predicted by the transfer-appropriate processing principle, priming in the category-association test was larger for words studied under the classification task than for those studied under the production task, whereas the opposite was true in the free-association test. This crossover dissociation is the first obtained between two implicit conceptual tests, and it provides support for the claim that it is necessary to distinguish between different types of conceptual processing.  相似文献   
449.
Different patterns of dissociation in unilateral spatial neglect   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
The aim of this paper is to discuss the issue of dissociations found in unilateral spatial neglect according to the modality of space exploration and the nature of the task. For this purpose we present a reanalysis of the data from a recent paper of Gentilini et al. (1989) comparing visual and blindfolded exploration of a computer keyboard and discuss the performance of a left-brain-damaged patient with right visuospatial neglect and left-sided neglect dyslexia. We conclude that unilateral spatial neglect cannot be interpreted as a disruption of a single attentional mechanism, but rather it reflects impaired attentional mechanisms at several levels of cognitive processing.  相似文献   
450.
Porntida Tanjitpiyanond  Jolanda Jetten  Kim Peters  Ashwini Ashokkumar  Oumar Barry  Matthew Billet  Maja Becker  Robert W. Booth  Diego Castro  Juana Chinchilla  Giulio Costantini  Egon Dejonckheere  Girts Dimdins  Yasemin Erbas  Agustín Espinosa  Gillian Finchilescu  Ángel Gómez  Roberto González  Nobuhiko Goto  Aya Hatano  Lea Hartwich  Somboon Jarukasemthawee  Jaya Kumar Karunagharan  Lindsay M. Novak  Jinseok P. Kim  Michal Kohút  Yi Liu  Steve Loughnan  Ike E. Onyishi  Charity N. Onyishi  Micaela Varela  Iris S. Pattara-angkoon  Müjde Peker  Kullaya Pisitsungkagarn  Muhammad Rizwan  Eunkook M. Suh  William Swann  Eddie M. W. Tong  Rhiannon N. Turner  Niels Vanhasbroeck  Paul A. M. Van Lange  Christin-Melanie Vauclair  Alexander Vinogradov  Grace Wacera  Zhechen Wang  Susilo Wibisono  Victoria Wai-Lan Yeung 《European journal of social psychology》2023,53(2):367-382
There is a growing body of work suggesting that social class stereotypes are amplified when people perceive higher levels of economic inequality—that is, the wealthy are perceived as more competent and assertive and the poor as more incompetent and unassertive. The present study tested this prediction in 32 societies and also examines the role of wealth-based categorization in explaining this relationship. We found that people who perceived higher economic inequality were indeed more likely to consider wealth as a meaningful basis for categorization. Unexpectedly, however, higher levels of perceived inequality were associated with perceiving the wealthy as less competent and assertive and the poor as more competent and assertive. Unpacking this further, exploratory analyses showed that the observed tendency to stereotype the wealthy negatively only emerged in societies with lower social mobility and democracy and higher corruption. This points to the importance of understanding how socio-structural features that co-occur with economic inequality may shape perceptions of the wealthy and the poor.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号