全文获取类型
收费全文 | 12001篇 |
免费 | 170篇 |
专业分类
12171篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 112篇 |
2019年 | 152篇 |
2018年 | 165篇 |
2017年 | 194篇 |
2016年 | 238篇 |
2015年 | 180篇 |
2014年 | 226篇 |
2013年 | 1241篇 |
2012年 | 389篇 |
2011年 | 423篇 |
2010年 | 289篇 |
2009年 | 289篇 |
2008年 | 400篇 |
2007年 | 436篇 |
2006年 | 437篇 |
2005年 | 383篇 |
2004年 | 354篇 |
2003年 | 371篇 |
2002年 | 372篇 |
2001年 | 168篇 |
2000年 | 195篇 |
1999年 | 170篇 |
1998年 | 191篇 |
1997年 | 171篇 |
1996年 | 172篇 |
1995年 | 160篇 |
1994年 | 161篇 |
1993年 | 138篇 |
1992年 | 151篇 |
1991年 | 150篇 |
1990年 | 128篇 |
1989年 | 127篇 |
1988年 | 132篇 |
1987年 | 140篇 |
1986年 | 141篇 |
1985年 | 149篇 |
1984年 | 153篇 |
1983年 | 171篇 |
1982年 | 186篇 |
1981年 | 173篇 |
1980年 | 158篇 |
1979年 | 140篇 |
1978年 | 176篇 |
1977年 | 169篇 |
1976年 | 154篇 |
1975年 | 152篇 |
1974年 | 152篇 |
1973年 | 146篇 |
1972年 | 109篇 |
1968年 | 82篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
941.
This study examined whether distinct groups of young adolescents with mainly anxiety or mainly depression could be identified in a general population sample. Latent class analysis was used on self-report ratings of DSM-IV symptoms of anxiety and depressive disorders, because it was hypothesized that these ratings provide a bigger chance to identify distinct groups than parent ratings of symptoms that are poorly associated with DSM-IV. Results from exploratory and confirmatory latent class analysis showed that only very small numbers of young adolescents had mainly anxiety or mainly depressive symptoms. Instead, a five-group model fitted the data best. These five groups contained young adolescents who either had a high, intermediate, or low probability to have comorbid symptoms of anxiety and depression. It was concluded that symptoms of DSM-IV anxiety and depressive disorders co-occur in young adolescents, and that latent class analysis on items that capture also severe symptoms like suicidal thoughts are needed to derive groups with specific comorbidity patterns in a general population sample. 相似文献
942.
Large-scale neural network for sentence processing 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Cooke A Grossman M DeVita C Gonzalez-Atavales J Moore P Chen W Gee J Detre J 《Brain and language》2006,96(1):14-36
Our model of sentence comprehension includes at least grammatical processes important for structure-building, and executive resources such as working memory that support these grammatical processes. We hypothesized that a core network of brain regions supports grammatical processes, and that additional brain regions are activated depending on the working memory demands associated with processing a particular grammatical feature. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to test this hypothesis by comparing cortical activation patterns during coherence judgments of sentences with three different syntactic features. We found activation of the ventral portion of left inferior frontal cortex during judgments of violations of each grammatical feature. Increased recruitment of the dorsal portion of left inferior frontal cortex was seen during judgments of violations of specific grammatical features that appear to involve a more prominent working memory component. Left posterolateral temporal cortex and anterior cingulate were also implicated in judging some of the grammatical features. Our observations are consistent with a large-scale neural network for sentence processing that includes a core set of regions for detecting and repairing several different kinds of grammatical features, and additional regions that appear to participate depending on the working memory demands associated with processing a particular grammatical feature. 相似文献
943.
Wible CG Han SD Spencer MH Kubicki M Niznikiewicz MH Jolesz FA McCarley RW Nestor P 《Brain and language》2006,97(3):294-305
Semantic priming refers to a reduction in the reaction time to identify or make a judgment about a stimulus that has been immediately preceded by a semantically related word or picture and is thought to result from a partial overlap in the semantic associates of the two words. A semantic priming lexical decision task using spoken words was presented in event-related fMRI and behavioral paradigms. Word pairs varied in terms of semantic relatedness and the connectivity between associates. Thirteen right-handed subjects underwent fMRI imaging and 10 additional subjects were tested in a behavioral version of the semantic priming task. It was hypothesized priming would be greatest, reaction time fastest, and cortical activation reduced the most for related word pairs of high connectivity, followed by related word pairs of low connectivity, and then by unrelated word pairs. Behavioral and fMRI results confirmed these predictions. fMRI activity located primarily in bilateral posterior superior and middle temporal regions showed modulation by connectivity and relatedness. The results suggest that these regions are involved in semantic processing. 相似文献
944.
A telephone survey of curriculum directors of the thirty institutes of the American Psychoanalytic Association revealed that 75 percent of institutes highly value the importance of evaluating what candidates learn from the curriculum. Paradoxically, however, most institutes do not do evaluations in this area. Typically, institutes evaluate candidates' satisfaction with courses, or their attendance and conduct in classes, rather than learning per se. Some institutes have more rigorous evaluation methods, including oral exams. Objections to evaluating what candidates learn included (1) evaluations involve too much work; (2) evaluations of learning are considered inappropriate or invalid; (3) evaluations would hurt candidates, faculty, the learning process, and the institute. Some curriculum directors dismissed these objections, contending that it is the responsibility of psychoanalytic educators to set teaching goals and evaluate learning. 相似文献
945.
Robert Logie Elizabeth Maylor Sergio Della Sala Geoff Smith 《Journal of Cognitive Psychology》2013,25(3):441-456
An experiment is reported examining the role of working memory in two laboratory‐based prospective memory (PM) tasks. Participants viewed a film for a later recognition memory task while simultaneously monitoring auditorially presented arithmetic problems for incorrect solutions. The arithmetic verification task was either low demand or high demand. In addition, participants were required either to indicate whenever an animal appeared in the film (event‐based PM task), or whenever 3 min had elapsed (time‐based PM task). PM performance was higher when the arithmetic task was low demand than when it was high demand. Young participants were more successful in both PM tasks than older participants, but only under high demand. Age did not interact with PM task type overall, and the young participants were faster overall in both types of PM task. Taken together, the results indicate that working memory plays an important role in PM tasks. 相似文献
946.
Robert C. Scharff 《British Journal for the History of Philosophy》2013,21(1):122-142
Wilhelm Dilthey is, famously, an epistemological pioneer for a second, ‘human’ kind of science that ‘understands’ life as we live it, instead of ‘explaining’ things as we observe them. Even today, he is usually cited for his role in the Erklären–Verstehen debate. My article, however, follows Heidegger's suggestion that we make the existence of the debate itself the problem. Whether there are different sorts of entity, different reasons for studying them and different means for doing so – such issues raise questions about science itself, not just about how to do it better. Moreover, what sort of philosopher is competent to address such questions? Heidegger argues that Dilthey's later writings intimate that it must be one who thinks from the ‘standpoint of (historical) life itself.’ This issue, says Heidegger, is ‘alive’ in Dilthey but is continually short-circuited by his very traditional plan for a ‘Critique of Historical Reason.’ Dilthey's unsuccessful struggles to produce this Critique are his gift to us, however. They encourage us to explicitly reconsider, as Heidegger does not only in Being and Time but throughout his life, what Dilthey cannot: If philosophy, like all human practices, is historical to the core, what is it to ‘be’ philosophical, about science or anything else? 相似文献
947.
James Robert Brown 《International Journal of Philosophical Studies》2013,21(1):111-115
The Semantic Tradition from Kant to Carnap: To the Vienna Station By J. Alberto Coffa, Cambridge University Press, 1991. Pp. xi + 445 ISBN 0–521–37429–4 £35.00. A Companion to Aristotle's Politics Edited by David Keyt and Fred D. Miller Jnr. Basil Blackwell, 1991. Pp. 407. ISBN 1–55786–096‐X. £14.95 pbk. Equality and Partiality By Thomas Nagel Oxford University Press, 1991. Pp. ix + 186. ISBN 0–19–506967–6. £12.95. 相似文献
948.
Husaini BA Cummings S Kilbourne B Roback H Sherkat D Levine R Cain VA 《International journal of group psychotherapy》2004,54(3):295-319
We describe and evaluate a group therapy program targeting depression among elderly residents (N=303) of subsidized high-rise apartments in Nashville, TN. This eclectic program was comprised of 12 sessions (a total of 24 hours) that included modules on exercise and preventive health behaviors, cognitive and re-motivation therapy, reminiscence and grief therapy, and social skills development. Our multivariate regression analyses of pre-post measures using the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) showed that the effects of the group therapy varied by race, age, and level of initial depression among the participants. The program was effective in reducing depression, but only among Caucasian women who reported at least moderate depression prior to the program, and it yielded greater benefits for women between 55 and 75 years of age. 相似文献
949.
The main purpose of this study was to identify whether a lot of sports training had any effect on the balance control associated with a leg movement. The nature of the training experience was also an important concern and we chose subject who had undergone specific training experience in absence of equilibrium constraints. To this end a comparison between control (untrained) subjects, triathletes and swimmers was designed to establish whether a general training in sports (triathletes) or a specific loadless training (swimmers), leads to differences in the balance control. A leg movement is preceded by a shift of the center of mass (CM) towards the supporting side to maintain equilibrium and forward to create the condition for progression. To provide an acceleration of the CM sideward and forward, an initial displacement of the center of pressure (CP) towards the moving limb and in posterior direction was performed. Interestingly, the lateral pressure onto the ground was greater increased in swimmers in both leg raising and obstacle avoidance tasks compared to the control group and/or triathletes whereas the backward CP shift in all group was the same. The initial control of the CM shift is very different in swimmers compared to triathletes and controls. The increased lateral pressure onto the ground in swimmers may be a result of a prolonged training in water. This suggests that prolonged training in the absence of equilibrium constraints has more of an effect on balance control than a prolonged general training. In addition, the lack of differences in the backward CP shift suggests that M/L and A/P controls support two functional goals: equilibrium maintenance and movement initiation. 相似文献
950.
Shea MT Stout RL Yen S Pagano ME Skodol AE Morey LC Gunderson JG McGlashan TH Grilo CM Sanislow CA Bender DS Zanarini MC 《Journal of abnormal psychology》2004,113(4):499-508
In this study, the authors examined time-varying associations between schizotypal (STPD), borderline (BPD), avoidant (AVPD), or obsessive-compulsive (OCPD) personality disorders and co-occurring Axis I disorders in 544 adult participants from the Collaborative Longitudinal Personality Disorders Study. The authors tested predictions of specific longitudinal associations derived from a model of crosscutting psychobiological dimensions (L. J. Siever & K. L. Davis, 1991) with participants with the relevant Axis I disorders. The authors assessed participants at baseline and at 6-, 12-, and 24-month follow-up evaluations. BPD showed significant longitudinal associations with major depressive disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder. AVPD was significantly associated with anxiety disorders (specifically social phobia and obsessive-compulsive disorder). Two of the four personality disorders under examination (STPD and OCPD) showed little or no association with Axis I disorders. 相似文献