全文获取类型
收费全文 | 67085篇 |
免费 | 1136篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
68224篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 770篇 |
2019年 | 935篇 |
2018年 | 1260篇 |
2017年 | 1288篇 |
2016年 | 1390篇 |
2015年 | 1003篇 |
2014年 | 1221篇 |
2013年 | 5808篇 |
2012年 | 2232篇 |
2011年 | 2256篇 |
2010年 | 1409篇 |
2009年 | 1418篇 |
2008年 | 1983篇 |
2007年 | 1977篇 |
2006年 | 1834篇 |
2005年 | 1538篇 |
2004年 | 1564篇 |
2003年 | 1511篇 |
2002年 | 1451篇 |
2001年 | 2071篇 |
2000年 | 1970篇 |
1999年 | 1488篇 |
1998年 | 776篇 |
1997年 | 678篇 |
1996年 | 745篇 |
1995年 | 682篇 |
1994年 | 677篇 |
1993年 | 634篇 |
1992年 | 1231篇 |
1991年 | 1141篇 |
1990年 | 1098篇 |
1989年 | 1065篇 |
1988年 | 1034篇 |
1987年 | 977篇 |
1986年 | 964篇 |
1985年 | 1025篇 |
1984年 | 856篇 |
1983年 | 755篇 |
1982年 | 632篇 |
1981年 | 604篇 |
1979年 | 868篇 |
1978年 | 711篇 |
1977年 | 609篇 |
1976年 | 597篇 |
1975年 | 729篇 |
1974年 | 811篇 |
1973年 | 825篇 |
1972年 | 700篇 |
1971年 | 632篇 |
1968年 | 679篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
931.
Eugene P. Trager M.D. 《Journal of religion and health》1996,35(2):117-123
The historical interplay between psychiatry and religion is placed in a contemporary context here. In their zeal to embrace
new discoveries and technologies in neurophysiology, modern psychiatrists are losing sight of spiritual and cultural dimensions.
From a biological perspective, there is confusion about what constitutes a cause, a concomitant, or a consequence of mental
illness. Computer metaphors are being used to explain mental illness. This article examines such trends and questions whether,
in the decade of the brain, psychiatry is in danger of losing its soul. 相似文献
932.
Inappropriate sexual behavior by professionals has been reported in increasing numbers. This article focuses on the cognitive-behavioral treatment of clergy who have acted out sexually. Case studies are used to identify the various issues and facets of treatment. The article concludes with a program designed to return previously offending clergy to active roles within their denominations is described. 相似文献
933.
Robert C. Fuller 《Pastoral Psychology》1996,44(6):371-383
The writings of the late Erik H. Erikson (1) have contributed directly to the psychological study of religion, (2) were amenable
to the efforts of others to develop normative theological arguments, and (3) might be seen as themselves examples of contemporary,
nontheological accounts of the religious dimension of human existence. This paper begins by reviewing the principal contributions
that Erikson made to the psychological study of religion, followed by a review of the uses that have been made of Erikson's
work for normative/constructive activities in such areas as practical theology and pastoral counseling. I will then argue
that Erikson's writings — when viewed in the vein of William James's radical empiricism and functionalist accounts of human
religiosity — identify an irreducibly religious dimension to normative human functioning. Erikson's functionalism constitutes
a form of nontheological religious thinking that speaks directly to concerns presenting themselves in contemporary culture. 相似文献
934.
ELIZABETH M. BAETEN 《Metaphilosophy》1996,27(4):408-425
The basic tenets of “classical” naturalism (exemplified in the work of Mead, Buchler, and Randall, among others) are delineated and distinguished from other versions of naturalism. Classical naturalism is also distinguished from reductive materialism and idealism. Nature is asserted to be indefinitely plural and not amenable to monistic or dualistic categorial schemes; that is, the principle of “ontological parity” is maintained. The method of inquiry of naturalism is outlined, along with the notion of truth as perspectivally objective. The metaphysical hypotheses are then used to examine culture. Culture is found to be the specific environment of human being, arising out of, but irreducible to, other natural processes. Culture is described as the cumulative gestures of the human social organism as it discovers and creates itself in mutual constitution with other natural processes and products. 相似文献
935.
936.
Decompositions and biplots in three-way correspondence analysis 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
In this paper correspondence analysis for three-way contingency tables is presented using three-way generalisations of the singular value decomposition. It is shown that in combination with Lancaster's (1951) additive decomposition of interactions in three-way tables, a detailed analysis is possible of the deviations from independence. Finally, biplots are shown to produce powerful graphical representations of the results from three-way correspondence analyses. An example from child development is used to illustrate the theoretical developments. 相似文献
937.
Robert N. Johnson 《The Southern journal of philosophy》1996,34(2):147-168
If any action is to be morally good it is not enough that it should conform to the moral law-it must also be done for the sake of the moral law: where this is not so, the conformity is only too contingent and precarious, since the nonmoral ground at work will now and then produce actions which accord with the law, but very often actions which transgress it. 相似文献
938.
Previous research has demonstrated that people have enormous difficulty in detecting distortions in such questions as, “How many animals of each kind did Moses take in the Ark?” Reder and Kusbit (1991) argued that the locus of the effect must be the existence of a partial-match process. Other research has suggested that this partial-match process operates at the word level and that, with adequate focus on the relevant word, the Moses illusion is greatly diminished. The present experimental results argue that those conclusions were based on a shift in response criterion with no concomitant change in ability to detect distortions. Furthermore, the data suggest that the matching process operates below the word level, at the level of distinctive features. 相似文献
939.
Sydney S. Zentall Arlene M. Hall David L. Lee 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1998,26(5):335-343
Mirrors have been used to focus attention to aspects of the self (e.g., to known strategies, standards). We hypothesized that this could be important for students with hyperactivity/inattention, who typically direct attention outward to external novelty. In this study, we administered a partially solvable word puzzle to 43 middle school students, with and without hyperactivity/inattention, in the presence and absence of a mirror, counterbalanced for condition and form order. Differences between students with hyperactivity/inattention and comparisons in accuracy were found only in the no mirror condition. Furthermore, the beneficial effect of the mirror for children with hyperactivity/inattention was most pronounced for those who looked at the mirror. Findings were interpreted in terms of their potential to remedy the production deficits of these children. 相似文献
940.
This is the second in a three-part study exploring the hypothesis that near-death experiences (NDErs) assign the meaning of the NDE by using causal (effect) and semantic (affect) attributions. To test this hypothesis, 32 spontaneous verbal accounts of NDEs were analyzed. Each statement comprising the account was coded and classified according to the six attributional types in the Norton-Sahlman matrices of attributional classification. On the bases of these findings, we conclude that NDErs abstract the most significant aspects of meaning from their experiences by the use of attributions expressing the purposes of the experience and the intentions of the participants. Second, the meaning and intensity of the experience derives from attributions of both effect (causality) and affective significance: the assignment of subjective meaning to objects and events (affect). Third, the findings demonstrate that there are significant changes in NDErs' overt and affective states, reinforcing our argument that meaning and intensity of the NDE is a function of how the experiencer assigns causation, in addition to the affective significance that the experiencer places on the events constituting the NDE. 相似文献