全文获取类型
收费全文 | 10838篇 |
免费 | 318篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
11158篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 102篇 |
2019年 | 134篇 |
2018年 | 150篇 |
2017年 | 176篇 |
2016年 | 220篇 |
2015年 | 162篇 |
2014年 | 197篇 |
2013年 | 1128篇 |
2012年 | 346篇 |
2011年 | 388篇 |
2010年 | 274篇 |
2009年 | 263篇 |
2008年 | 366篇 |
2007年 | 405篇 |
2006年 | 401篇 |
2005年 | 351篇 |
2004年 | 323篇 |
2003年 | 348篇 |
2002年 | 343篇 |
2001年 | 153篇 |
2000年 | 177篇 |
1999年 | 156篇 |
1998年 | 177篇 |
1997年 | 158篇 |
1996年 | 164篇 |
1995年 | 154篇 |
1994年 | 156篇 |
1993年 | 129篇 |
1992年 | 141篇 |
1991年 | 134篇 |
1990年 | 116篇 |
1989年 | 119篇 |
1988年 | 117篇 |
1987年 | 125篇 |
1986年 | 127篇 |
1985年 | 139篇 |
1984年 | 144篇 |
1983年 | 154篇 |
1982年 | 173篇 |
1981年 | 161篇 |
1980年 | 150篇 |
1979年 | 124篇 |
1978年 | 164篇 |
1977年 | 153篇 |
1976年 | 144篇 |
1975年 | 141篇 |
1974年 | 143篇 |
1973年 | 135篇 |
1972年 | 105篇 |
1968年 | 72篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
52.
In this paper, we discuss the potential of HyperCard for research and instruction in psychology. First we give a general overview of the HyperCard program; after that, we present two HyperCard stacks as sample solutions for two specific research applications. Surveyor, a self-contained survey tool, is a HyperCard-based vehicle for developing, administering, and processing tests and surveys. Queston demonstrates how HyperCard can be used as a data-management and data-analysis tool during the stages of questionnaire development. Both stacks illustrate how flexible HyperCard is and how easy it is to use it to manage, analyze, and process data, to transfer data to other programs, and to print reports. HyperCard, unlike traditional applications, gives the user a great degree of control over the way information is stored, mainipulated, and presented. Although both stacks are custom-made for specific purposes, the concepts underlying the design can be generally applied and adapted for other purposes. 相似文献
53.
Reinterpreting the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator From the Perspective of the Five-Factor Model of Personality 总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6
The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI; Myers & McCaulley, 1985) was evaluated from the perspectives of Jung's theory of psychological types and the five-factor model of personality as measured by self-reports and peer ratings on the NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI; Costa & McCrae, 1985b). Data were provided by 267 men and 201 women ages 19 to 93. Consistent with earlier research and evaluations, there was no support for the view that the MBTI measures truly dichotomous preferences or qualitatively distinct types; instead, the instrument measures four relatively independent dimensions. The interpretation of the Judging-Perceiving index was also called into question. The data suggest that Jung's theory is either incorrect or inadequately operationalized by the MBTI and cannot provide a sound basis for interpreting it. However, correlational analyses showed that the four MBTI indices did measure aspects of four of the five major dimensions of normal personality. The five-factor model provides an alternative basis for interpreting MBTI findings within a broader, more commonly shared conceptual framework. 相似文献
54.
55.
56.
Robert M. Baird 《Journal of religion and health》1985,24(2):117-124
Against the background claims that a meaningful life is more likely to be the product of creation than discovery and that a meaningful life must involve both subjective satisfaction and an objective moral component, three foundations of a meaningful life are discussed. These include the quality of one's relationships, the integrative power of one's goals and projects, and the role of story or stories. Concerning the role of story, the theological challenge is to choose that story or those stories that place life in a genuinely ultimate context. 相似文献
57.
Robert E Guttentag 《Developmental Review》1985,5(1):56-82
Research is reviewed suggesting that early school age children and older adults demonstrate a similar pattern of performance deficits (relative to the performance of young adults) on memory tasks, raising the possibility that age-related changes in some common factor might account for both memory development and memory decline. It is argued here that whereas “metamemory” theory of memory development does not provide a viable theory of memory decline, Craik's (F. I. M. Craik & M. Byrd, 1982, in F. I. M. Craik & S. E. Trehub (Eds.), Aging and cognitive processes, New York: Plenum; F. I. M. Craik & E. Simon, 1980, in L. W. Poon, J. L. Fozard, L. S. Cermak, D. Arenberg, & L. W. Thompson (Eds.), New directions in memory and aging, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum) attentional deficit theory of memory decline provides a viable theory of memory development as well. Evidence supporting this position is reviewed, and directions for future research are discussed. 相似文献
58.
59.
Robert W. Frick 《Memory & cognition》1985,13(4):346-356
Two experiments testing immediate ordered recall are presented; in these experiments, subjects engaged in repetitive speech (“articulatory suppression”) during a visual presentation in order to prevent auditory recoding of the stimuli. In both experiments, a simultaneous presentation produced results that suggested the use of visual short-term memory, whereas a sequential presentation did not. In Experiment 1, visual confusion errors occurred more often than would be expected by chance for a simultaneous presentation but not for a sequential presentation. In Experiment 2, recall from visual short-term memory was expected to suffer more when subjects wrote a prefix than when they spoke a prefix; this effect occurred for a simultaneous presentation but not for a sequential presentation. These results suggest that existence of a visual short term store that retains a simultaneous presentation but not a sequential presentation. 相似文献
60.
Previous publications emanating from the Colorado Adoption Project have reported significant relationships between parental general cognitive ability (g) and infant Bayley MDI scores. The present study compared infant Bayley factor scores representing separable dimensions of infant cognition with parental general and specific cognitive abilities for 182 adoptive families and 164 nonadoptive families. Parent/offspring correlations between 12-month Bayley factors and parental cognitive abilities suggest only minimal relationships for both parental g and specific abilities. At 24 months, more parent/offspring resemblance was present; moreover, Bayley factors that were related to parental cognition tended to be related to parental g, not to specific abilities. The finding of significant parent/offspring relationships at 24 months between biological parents and their adopted-away infants, as well as between nonadoptive parents and their infants, suggests some genetic continuity from infancy to adulthood. 相似文献