首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   172406篇
  免费   7735篇
  国内免费   156篇
  2021年   1516篇
  2020年   2804篇
  2019年   3481篇
  2018年   3506篇
  2017年   3991篇
  2016年   4651篇
  2015年   3957篇
  2014年   4847篇
  2013年   23700篇
  2012年   4564篇
  2011年   3665篇
  2010年   3925篇
  2009年   4824篇
  2008年   3851篇
  2007年   3443篇
  2006年   4067篇
  2005年   4040篇
  2004年   3532篇
  2003年   3213篇
  2002年   3000篇
  2001年   3157篇
  2000年   3028篇
  1999年   3061篇
  1998年   2843篇
  1997年   2688篇
  1996年   2593篇
  1995年   2431篇
  1994年   2405篇
  1993年   2348篇
  1992年   2527篇
  1991年   2405篇
  1990年   2225篇
  1989年   2154篇
  1988年   2160篇
  1987年   2167篇
  1986年   2138篇
  1985年   2358篇
  1984年   2496篇
  1983年   2302篇
  1982年   2395篇
  1981年   2350篇
  1980年   2192篇
  1979年   2172篇
  1978年   2177篇
  1977年   2143篇
  1976年   1953篇
  1975年   1966篇
  1974年   2031篇
  1973年   1906篇
  1972年   1482篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
The purpose of this study is to compare field investigative interviews of children (FIIC) with three different legal outcomes in child sexual abuse cases: (i) insufficient evidence to proceed (IEP); (ii) convictions; or (iii) acquittals by the court. One hundred FIIC were divided into one of the three outcome possibilities. Amongst the female interviewees older than 10 years, there were no cases of acquittals and the convicted cases were over-represented. The children's response to open questions was found to be the main difference between the three FIIC outcomes. The responses to these open questions were 1.9 and 2.3 times longer in the convicted cases compared to acquittals and IEP. Possible explanations for the result are discussed. Copyright © 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
52.
53.
54.
Adult age differences in the consistency effect were examined in 3 experiments. The consistency effect refers to items inconsistent with expectations being better remembered than items consistent with expectations. Younger and older adults walked into an office room and viewed objects that varied in their consistency with expectation. Immediate and delayed recognition tests on item information (i.e., distractors were defined by their semantic identity) revealed that both age groups recognized unexpected items better than expected items. However, when recognition of token information was requested (i.e., distractors were defined by their physical appearance), younger adults, in contrast to older adults, exhibited consistency effects. Also, under divided attention, young adults revealed the same pattern of data as did elderly adults under full attention. The results are discussed in terms of capacity-related differences in distinctive encoding.  相似文献   
55.
We investigated motivational and cognitive processes of behavior change with respect to mammography screening. One hundred forty-two women (ages 40 and older) recruited from three worksites answered a 41-item questionnaire consisting of statements based on constructs from the transtheoretical model of behavior change. Principal-components analysis identified two factors: a six-item component representing positive perceptions of mammography (Pros) and a six-item component representing avoidance of mammography (Cons). Analysis of variance showed that Pros, Cons, and a derived Decisional Balance measure (Pros minus Cons) were associated with stage of mammography adoption. Results are consistent with applications of the model to smoking cessation. The model is also discussed as it relates to other theories of behavior change and as a general strategy for analyzing perceptual data pertinent to health-related actions and intentions for behavioral change.  相似文献   
56.
Range of electric vehicles (EVs) has long been considered a major barrier in acceptance of electric mobility. We examined the nature of how range is experienced in an EV and whether variables from other adaptation contexts, notably stress, have explanatory power for inter‐individual differences in what we term comfortable range. Forty EVs were leased to a sample of users for a 6‐month field study. Qualitative and quantitative analyses of range experiences were performed, including regression analyses to examine the role of stress‐buffering personality traits and coping skills in comfortable range. Users appraised range as a resource to which they could successfully adapt and that satisfied most of their daily mobility needs. However, indicators were found that suggested suboptimal range utilisation. Stress‐buffering personality traits (control beliefs, ambiguity tolerance) and coping skills (subjective range competence, daily range practice) were found to play a substantial role in comfortable range. Hence, it may be possible to overcome perceived range barriers with the assistance of psychological interventions such as information, training, and interface design. Providing drivers with a reliable usable range may be more important than enhancing maximal range in an electric mobility system.  相似文献   
57.
58.
59.
60.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号