首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   30662篇
  免费   324篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2020年   102篇
  2019年   134篇
  2018年   3587篇
  2017年   2936篇
  2016年   2427篇
  2015年   344篇
  2014年   265篇
  2013年   1187篇
  2012年   887篇
  2011年   2691篇
  2010年   2707篇
  2009年   1682篇
  2008年   2006篇
  2007年   2495篇
  2006年   421篇
  2005年   547篇
  2004年   477篇
  2003年   455篇
  2002年   399篇
  2001年   181篇
  2000年   223篇
  1999年   171篇
  1998年   197篇
  1997年   174篇
  1996年   171篇
  1995年   154篇
  1994年   157篇
  1993年   129篇
  1992年   141篇
  1991年   134篇
  1990年   124篇
  1989年   120篇
  1988年   117篇
  1987年   125篇
  1986年   127篇
  1985年   139篇
  1984年   144篇
  1983年   155篇
  1982年   174篇
  1981年   161篇
  1980年   150篇
  1979年   124篇
  1978年   166篇
  1977年   153篇
  1976年   147篇
  1975年   142篇
  1974年   144篇
  1973年   137篇
  1972年   106篇
  1968年   73篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 656 毫秒
231.
232.
233.
234.
235.
When 72 male students in a southern theological institution were compared by the manner in which they intended to fulfill their religious vocation (i.e., as missionaries, pastors, or religious education workers), a homogeneity of personality behavior was observed on each of 16 factors. The total sample, however, was reported to be substantially and significantly different from the general population on four factors. No significant difference was reported among the three groups in their need for social approval as measured by the Marlowe-Crowne SDS.  相似文献   
236.
When S looks at a visual target through prisms, adaptive shifts in reaching behavior occur even though he sees no part of his body through the prisms. These shifts are caused by a change in the judgment of the direction of gaze (oculomotor change), which in turn is caused by two secondary prismatic effects: (a) asymmetry of the visual display and (b) apparent rotation about a vertical axis of a panel or wall facing S. The “asymmetry” factor contributes 22% of the total oculomotor change, and the “rotation” effect contributes the remaining 78%. Oculomotor change is not facilitated by eye-movement activity. The adaptive oculomotor change induces a non-adaptive proprioception change about one-tenth as large as the oculomotor change. These findings are capable of accounting for the previously unexplained results reported by Wooster in 1923, and also for the current controversy about the role of reafferent stimulation in sensorymotor adaptation.  相似文献   
237.
Binocular fusion may be due to interocular inhibitory suppression, an hypothesis difficult to evaluate by phenomenal inspection. A test probe method (reaction time to a light pulse) was used to measure visual sensitivity during binocular rivalry and fusion. The absence of inhibitory effects during fusion fails to support the suppression theory of fusion.  相似文献   
238.
When S looks at a visual target through prisms, adaptive shifts in reaching behavior occur even though he sees no part of his body through the prisms. These shifts are caused by a change in the judgment of the direction of gaze (oculomotor change), which in turn is caused by two secondary prismatic effects: (a) asymmetry of the visual display and (b) apparent rotation about a vertical axis of a panel or wall facing S. The “asymmetry” factor contributes 22% of the total oculomotor change, and the “rotation” effect contributes the remaining 78%. Oculomotor change is not facilitated by eye-movzment activity. The adaptive oculomotor change induces a non-adaptive proprioception change about one-tenth as large as the oculomotor change. These findings are capable of accounting for the previously unexplained results reported by Wooster in 1923, and also for the current controversy about the role of reafferent stimulation in sensorymotor adaptation.  相似文献   
239.
240.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号