首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   51878篇
  免费   1046篇
  国内免费   13篇
  2019年   341篇
  2018年   3836篇
  2017年   3210篇
  2016年   2728篇
  2015年   561篇
  2014年   553篇
  2013年   2761篇
  2012年   1446篇
  2011年   3288篇
  2010年   3054篇
  2009年   2034篇
  2008年   2513篇
  2007年   2997篇
  2006年   883篇
  2005年   963篇
  2004年   893篇
  2003年   825篇
  2002年   826篇
  2001年   884篇
  2000年   949篇
  1999年   709篇
  1998年   438篇
  1997年   374篇
  1996年   359篇
  1994年   335篇
  1992年   576篇
  1991年   530篇
  1990年   555篇
  1989年   529篇
  1988年   509篇
  1987年   512篇
  1986年   520篇
  1985年   548篇
  1984年   465篇
  1983年   440篇
  1982年   396篇
  1981年   364篇
  1979年   498篇
  1978年   430篇
  1977年   396篇
  1976年   354篇
  1975年   474篇
  1974年   508篇
  1973年   476篇
  1972年   426篇
  1971年   355篇
  1969年   382篇
  1968年   426篇
  1967年   364篇
  1966年   375篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
901.
When S looks at a visual target through prisms, adaptive shifts in reaching behavior occur even though he sees no part of his body through the prisms. These shifts are caused by a change in the judgment of the direction of gaze (oculomotor change), which in turn is caused by two secondary prismatic effects: (a) asymmetry of the visual display and (b) apparent rotation about a vertical axis of a panel or wall facing S. The “asymmetry” factor contributes 22% of the total oculomotor change, and the “rotation” effect contributes the remaining 78%. Oculomotor change is not facilitated by eye-movement activity. The adaptive oculomotor change induces a non-adaptive proprioception change about one-tenth as large as the oculomotor change. These findings are capable of accounting for the previously unexplained results reported by Wooster in 1923, and also for the current controversy about the role of reafferent stimulation in sensorymotor adaptation.  相似文献   
902.
Binocular fusion may be due to interocular inhibitory suppression, an hypothesis difficult to evaluate by phenomenal inspection. A test probe method (reaction time to a light pulse) was used to measure visual sensitivity during binocular rivalry and fusion. The absence of inhibitory effects during fusion fails to support the suppression theory of fusion.  相似文献   
903.
Voluntary attention to one of two static objects in the peripheral field of one eye makes this object more liable to masking by a moving object in the corresponding area of the field of the other eye (Experiment 1).

Positive after images (and probably negative after images) are subject to (binocular) movement masking (Experiment 2).

Movement masking can occur in the field of either eye, but with the displays so far tried the inhibitory influence of a moving object is less in the field of the eye to which it is shown than in the field of the other eye (Experiment 3).  相似文献   
904.
Observers attempted to detect the presence of a pitch difference between two successive tones. The percentage of correct judgments was equivalent for tones separated by .95, 4.5, and 8.9 sec. There was a general increase in reports of a pitch difference with increased intertone interval, which is interpreted as arising from hypothesized shifts in the neural locus of the first stimulus during the intertone interval.  相似文献   
905.
Visual and acoustic confusability between a target item and background items was varied in a visual search task. Visual confusability was a highly significant source of difficulty while acoustic confusability had no effect. The results do not seem to be interpretable within a theory which assumes compulsory auditory encoding of visual information.  相似文献   
906.
When S looks at a visual target through prisms, adaptive shifts in reaching behavior occur even though he sees no part of his body through the prisms. These shifts are caused by a change in the judgment of the direction of gaze (oculomotor change), which in turn is caused by two secondary prismatic effects: (a) asymmetry of the visual display and (b) apparent rotation about a vertical axis of a panel or wall facing S. The “asymmetry” factor contributes 22% of the total oculomotor change, and the “rotation” effect contributes the remaining 78%. Oculomotor change is not facilitated by eye-movzment activity. The adaptive oculomotor change induces a non-adaptive proprioception change about one-tenth as large as the oculomotor change. These findings are capable of accounting for the previously unexplained results reported by Wooster in 1923, and also for the current controversy about the role of reafferent stimulation in sensorymotor adaptation.  相似文献   
907.
908.
909.
910.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号