全文获取类型
收费全文 | 479篇 |
免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
482篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 13篇 |
2019年 | 8篇 |
2018年 | 6篇 |
2017年 | 23篇 |
2016年 | 13篇 |
2015年 | 9篇 |
2014年 | 9篇 |
2013年 | 56篇 |
2012年 | 22篇 |
2011年 | 15篇 |
2010年 | 14篇 |
2009年 | 15篇 |
2008年 | 15篇 |
2007年 | 21篇 |
2006年 | 7篇 |
2005年 | 16篇 |
2004年 | 16篇 |
2003年 | 10篇 |
2002年 | 14篇 |
2001年 | 3篇 |
2000年 | 7篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 8篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 8篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 10篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有482条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
This study investigated the role of neutral, happy, fearful, and angry facial expressions in enhancing orienting to the direction of eye gaze. Photographs of faces with either direct or averted gaze were presented. A target letter (T or L) appeared unpredictably to the left or the right of the face, either 300 ms or 700 ms after gaze direction changed. Response times were faster in congruent conditions (i.e., when the eyes gazed toward the target) relative to incongruent conditions (when the eyes gazed away from the target letter). Facial expression did influence reaction times, but these effects were qualified by individual differences in self-reported anxiety. High trait-anxious participants showed an enhanced orienting to the eye gaze of faces with fearful expressions relative to all other expressions. In contrast, when the eyes stared straight ahead, trait anxiety was associated with slower responding when the facial expressions depicted anger. Thus, in anxiety-prone people attention is more likely to be held by an expression of anger, whereas attention is guided more potently by fearful facial expressions. 相似文献
52.
Cornoldi C Bassani C Berto R Mammarella N 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2007,14(1):1-21
This study investigated the active component of visuo-spatial working memory (VSWM) in younger and older adults testing the hypotheses that elderly individuals have a poorer performance than younger ones and that errors in active VSWM tasks depend, at least partially, on difficulties in avoiding intrusions (i.e., avoiding already activated information). In two experiments, participants were presented with sequences of matrices on which three positions were pointed out sequentially: their task was to process all the positions but indicate only the final position of each sequence. Results showed a poorer performance in the elderly compared to the younger group and a higher number of intrusion (errors due to activated but irrelevant positions) rather than invention (errors consisting of pointing out a position never indicated by the experiementer) errors. The number of errors increased when a concurrent task was introduced (Experiment 1) and it was affected by different patterns of matrices (Experiment 2). In general, results show that elderly people have an impaired VSWM and produce a large number of errors due to inhibition failures. However, both the younger and the older adults' visuo-spatial working memory was affected by the presence of activated irrelevant information, the reduction of the available resources, and task constraints. 相似文献
53.
Dr. Jeffrey M. Halperin Rachel Gittelman Donald F. Klein Rita G. Rudel 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1984,12(1):1-14
Mixed hyperactive/reading-disabled children, selected from a large sample of hyperactive children, were compared to pure hyperactive children on demographic, behavioral, and neuropsychological measures. Children in the Mixed group were reading-disabled relative to age and IQ; those in the Pure group had achievement scores that were average relative to age and IQ. The Mixed group was significantly older than the Pure group; there were no differences in gender, race, or socioeconomic status. Behaviorally, the groups did not differ significantly on teacher ratings or on psychiatric ratings of aggression. The Mixed group had a significantly higher Performance IQ, whereas the Pure group had a significantly higher Verbal IQ and performed better on measures of cognitive impulsivity. Several other measures failed to distinguish the groups. The data give minimal support to the notion that pure hyperactive and mixed hyperative/reading-disabled children constitute distinct subgroups of Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity.This research was supported in part by Mental Health Clinical Research Grant MH-30906 and by U. S. Public Health Service Grant MH-18579. 相似文献
54.
Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and the radial arm maze: Spatial memory and serial position effects
M Craig J Rand R Mesch M Shyan-Norwalt J Morton E Flickinger 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》2012,126(3):233-242
The present study investigated spatial memory in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) through the use of a radial arm maze. The study consisted of a total of three separate experiments. In the first two experiments, the ability of the dogs to successfully remember previously unentered arms was evaluated. The third experiment was similar to the first two, but also examined the nature of the serial position effect. Performance in all three experiments was better than expected solely by random choices. Dogs showed a much better memory for spatial locations presented earlier in a spatial list compared with those presented in the middle. Based on the present results, we suggest that the radial arm maze assesses canine spatial memory and that dogs show a primacy effect. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved). 相似文献
55.
56.
Espelage Dorothy L. Valido Alberto Robinson Luz E. Ingram Katherine M. El Sheikh America Woolweaver Ashley M. Koritz Laura Vincent Claudia G. Marquez Brion Walker Hill M. Svanks Rita Marmolejos Roslyn Reynoso Medina Courtney Meltsner Zeke Yalamanchi Kovida Pennefather Jordan 《Social Psychology of Education》2022,25(5):1177-1203
Social Psychology of Education - Creating effective school-wide safety mechanisms depends on understanding students' previous experiences of reporting, however, students may also be... 相似文献
57.
Onochie Ike Okoye Ferdinand Maduka-Okafor Rita Chimuanya Matthias Anthonia Udeaja Abali I. Chuku 《Journal of Academic Ethics》2018,16(3):275-285
Concern is growing as research continues to find evidence of academic misconduct among medical students. There is, however, paucity of information on this issue among medical students and medical graduates in Africa. We determined the perceptions and attitude of house officers on academic misconduct within Nigerian medical schools. We conducted 7 focus group discussions (FGDs) among pre-registration house-officers (PRHOs) working in a Nigerian Teaching hospital between October and November 2013. A FGD guide containing 7 broad questions related to their perception and attitude on academic misconduct was employed. Ethnographic content analysis and manual coding were adopted in identifying themes, issues and representative quotations. A total of 58 participants out of an eligible pool of 76 PRHOs were recruited. All the groups associated academic misconduct chiefly with cheating in examinations, and perceived it as a commonplace problem. “It is a part of school life”, admitted one PRHO. Copying during exam appeared to be the commonest form of cheating. Another respondent opined that “The lecturers should be more concerned with helping students understand better. They should not make passing an exam a do-or-die affair.” The perception and attitudes towards academic misconduct may be a reflection of the prevailing moral decline in the contemporary Nigerian society. Underlying the reasons adduced by the participants for getting involved in academic misconduct is the fear of failure in examinations. Apart from consolidating formal and informal medical ethics education, the medical schools should ensure that the opportunities and pressures to indulge in academic misconduct are minimized. 相似文献
58.
59.
60.
After briefly describing the nature of emotional processing biases associated with vulnerability to anxiety, and a model of how they may be produced, we review new data on the experimental induction of attentional and interpretative biases. We show that these biases can be readily induced in the laboratory, in the absence of mood changes. However, induced biases exert effects on the processing of new information and cause congruent changes in state anxiety when they influence how emotionally significant information is encoded. We can therefore conclude that biases have causal effects on vulnerability to anxiety via their influence on how significant events are processed. Finally, we discuss how our model might account for the acquisition of processing bias and for when they can influence anxiety. 相似文献