首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   107149篇
  免费   4442篇
  国内免费   53篇
  2020年   1238篇
  2019年   1491篇
  2018年   2135篇
  2017年   2135篇
  2016年   2245篇
  2015年   1588篇
  2014年   1925篇
  2013年   9139篇
  2012年   3499篇
  2011年   3578篇
  2010年   2223篇
  2009年   2230篇
  2008年   3219篇
  2007年   3194篇
  2006年   2856篇
  2005年   2547篇
  2004年   2528篇
  2003年   2332篇
  2002年   2338篇
  2001年   3441篇
  2000年   3297篇
  1999年   2519篇
  1998年   1290篇
  1997年   1110篇
  1996年   1117篇
  1995年   1049篇
  1994年   1019篇
  1993年   1040篇
  1992年   2044篇
  1991年   1921篇
  1990年   1909篇
  1989年   1752篇
  1988年   1716篇
  1987年   1623篇
  1986年   1630篇
  1985年   1725篇
  1984年   1442篇
  1983年   1245篇
  1982年   984篇
  1979年   1458篇
  1978年   1086篇
  1975年   1233篇
  1974年   1391篇
  1973年   1381篇
  1972年   1188篇
  1971年   1085篇
  1969年   970篇
  1968年   1213篇
  1967年   1109篇
  1966年   1024篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
42.
Convergent and divergent stereo mechanisms were compared in their ability to recover structure from motion. Contrary to a recent result reported by Richards and Lieberman, no difference in their performance was found; both mechanisms appeared equally capable of supporting the perception of good structure from motion. Possible reasons for the disparate results are discussed.  相似文献   
43.
44.
This paper was not available at the time of going to press. It will be published in a future volume of the Proceedings of The Aristotelian Society.  相似文献   
45.
The three reliability and two construct validity studies presented here represent the first psychometric research on the commercially successful and popular Hartman Color Code Personality Profile. The reliability studies found high reliability after 3 and 6 weeks. The construct validity studies substantiated that the Color Code assesses some personality traits. The magnitude of the error variance suggests caution, however, in using the Color Code classifications at the individual level.  相似文献   
46.
47.
It is widely assumed that reinforcers are biologically relevant stimuli, or stimuli that have been associated with biologically relevant stimuli. However, brief, arbitrary stimuli have also been reported to have reinforcement-like effects, despite being unrelated to biologically relevant stimuli like food. The present study explored the potential reinforcement-like effects of brief stimuli across 5 experiments. In Experiments 1 through 4, pigeon subjects responded for food reinforcement and brief stimulus presentations in a 2-component multiple schedule. Neither baseline response rates nor resistance to change during disruption tests were systematically greater in a component with versus without brief stimulus presentations. Increasing the rate and duration of brief stimulus presentations in Experiment 4 did not reveal reinforcement-like effects when compared directly with food. In Experiment 5, pigeons chose between independent terminal links in a concurrent-chains procedure. Across conditions, varying the location, duration, and rate of brief stimulus presentations in the terminal links had no systematic effects on preference. In contrast, varying rates of food reinforcers resulted in large and reliable shifts in preference. Therefore, the present study found no systematic evidence that brief stimuli unrelated to food reliably increase response rates, resistance to change, or preference. These data demonstrate the value of systematic replication, and a behavioral momentum approach to assessing potential reinforcement-like effects.  相似文献   
48.
In three experiments, successive negative contrast was examined in one-way avoidance learning. Reward magnitude in first (pre-shift) and second (post-shift) phases was manipulated by time spent in the safe compartment. Experiment 1 demonstrated that when time in the danger compartment was held constant, a group shifted from a large reward--30 sec spent in the safe compartment--to a small reward--1 sec--showed poor performance and longer response latency than a group conditioned with the small reward in both phases. Experiment 2 replicated this effect with a less intense shock and also demonstrated that a group shifted from large to small reward performed more poorly than a group exposed to large reward--30 sec--in both phases. Finally, Experiment 3 showed that changes in intertrial interval, defined as total time spent in the safe compartment and the danger compartment before the onset of the warning signal, were not responsible for this contrast effect. These results suggest that time spent in a safe place can act as appetitive incentive during one-way avoidance learning.  相似文献   
49.
50.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号