首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   8135篇
  免费   271篇
  国内免费   5篇
  2020年   86篇
  2019年   93篇
  2018年   146篇
  2017年   143篇
  2016年   174篇
  2015年   134篇
  2014年   135篇
  2013年   830篇
  2012年   279篇
  2011年   292篇
  2010年   192篇
  2009年   224篇
  2008年   288篇
  2007年   320篇
  2006年   292篇
  2005年   272篇
  2004年   258篇
  2003年   318篇
  2002年   277篇
  2001年   125篇
  2000年   119篇
  1999年   140篇
  1998年   107篇
  1997年   122篇
  1996年   113篇
  1995年   130篇
  1994年   102篇
  1993年   119篇
  1992年   105篇
  1991年   97篇
  1990年   81篇
  1989年   86篇
  1988年   90篇
  1987年   103篇
  1986年   84篇
  1985年   95篇
  1984年   119篇
  1983年   124篇
  1982年   111篇
  1981年   109篇
  1980年   112篇
  1979年   112篇
  1978年   119篇
  1977年   107篇
  1976年   120篇
  1975年   97篇
  1974年   100篇
  1973年   93篇
  1972年   62篇
  1971年   54篇
排序方式: 共有8411条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
131.
The minimal transformation to orthonormality   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
The solution is presented to the problem of finding that orthonormal matrix closest in the least-square sense to a given matrix of full rank. An application is shown to the problem in multiple regression analysis of determining the importance of each independent variable.  相似文献   
132.
The relationship between the occupational preferences of 147 superior students and their fathers' occupations was traced over the 4-year period that the students attended high schools in Wisconsin. Analysis of the data indicated that both male and female superior students tended to state vocational preferences at the professional level early in high school and to maintain this preference throughout. Their occupational preferences were generally at a higher level than those of their fathers. There was no trend away from general preferences for work at a professional level toward naming of specific occupations within that level. The results suggest that theories of vocational development that imply that stages are passed through during later adolescent periods do not apply to the superior student population of this study.  相似文献   
133.
In 11 male albino rats, lever-pressing responses, maintained under a CRF escape schedule with light as the aversive stimulus, were examined at each of five intensities, viz., 2.5, 18, 105, 190, and 386 ft-C. The function relating reciprocal of latency of the escape response to aversive light intensity passed through a maximum.  相似文献   
134.
Graphic designers and an experimental psychologist worked together to improve the design of two map symbols which are frequently confused: the symbols for cuttings and embankments on topographic maps. The problem was analysed in terms of the function of the symbols and their likely cognitive representations. Tests were developed to evaluate alternative designs, including an intervisibility task which requirred users to visualize the landform from the symbols viewed in the context of a map. Tests were given to schoolchildren and to experienced map users in order to compare the standard symbols with five alternative designs. Children's performance was strongly affected by the symbols they used, but experienced users were much less affected. After some refinement of the symbols a further experiment demonstrated the superiority of a number of alternative designs over the existing symbols on a range of test: scores were almost double on the intervisibility task. The paper makes recommendations to cartographers and argues for greater consideration of the inexperienced map user in the design process.  相似文献   
135.
136.
137.
138.
A new approach is described for the development and structure of perceptual knowledge-based systems. This systematic method for acquiring perceptual knowledge for use in knowledge-based systems and for representing perceptual knowledge within the systems has revealed that two broad classes of perceptual activity can each be characterized by a single logical operator. The number of rules necessary to accomplish particular perceptual tasks can also be estimated.  相似文献   
139.
140.
The present paper is a commentary on an article by Drew Leder [1]. Leder identifies a series of texts in the clinical encounter, emphasizes the central role of interpretation in making sense of each of these texts, and articulates ordering principles to guide the interpretive work.The metaphor of clinical work as textual explication, however, creates the expectation that there is a text somewhere to be found. Such an expectation invites doctors and patients to search for the text and runs the risk of conceptualizing patients as more static than they are. If one is to use the textual metaphor, one must appreciate the radical extent to which the clinical encounter is a mutually produced and shifting entity. The qualities of mutuality and indeterminacy are not those one usually associates with texts. One might ultimately be better served by a different metaphor based more directly on uncertainty.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号