全文获取类型
收费全文 | 45681篇 |
免费 | 724篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
46407篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 522篇 |
2019年 | 645篇 |
2018年 | 853篇 |
2017年 | 926篇 |
2016年 | 965篇 |
2015年 | 681篇 |
2014年 | 807篇 |
2013年 | 3648篇 |
2012年 | 1512篇 |
2011年 | 1497篇 |
2010年 | 998篇 |
2009年 | 969篇 |
2008年 | 1361篇 |
2007年 | 1406篇 |
2006年 | 1241篇 |
2005年 | 1119篇 |
2004年 | 1056篇 |
2003年 | 1059篇 |
2002年 | 1044篇 |
2001年 | 1260篇 |
2000年 | 1217篇 |
1999年 | 994篇 |
1998年 | 465篇 |
1997年 | 482篇 |
1996年 | 448篇 |
1995年 | 466篇 |
1993年 | 447篇 |
1992年 | 827篇 |
1991年 | 734篇 |
1990年 | 768篇 |
1989年 | 669篇 |
1988年 | 720篇 |
1987年 | 682篇 |
1986年 | 670篇 |
1985年 | 627篇 |
1984年 | 601篇 |
1983年 | 581篇 |
1982年 | 445篇 |
1980年 | 438篇 |
1979年 | 664篇 |
1978年 | 505篇 |
1977年 | 441篇 |
1975年 | 531篇 |
1974年 | 583篇 |
1973年 | 616篇 |
1972年 | 452篇 |
1971年 | 432篇 |
1969年 | 432篇 |
1968年 | 512篇 |
1967年 | 465篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
861.
Word repetitions in sentence recognition 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
When some items on a list are strengthened by extra study time or repetitions, recognition of other, unrelated, list items is not harmed (Ratcliff, Clark, & Shiffrin, 1990). Shiffrin, Ratcliff, and Clark (1990) accounted for this list-strength finding with a model assuming that different items are stored separately in memory, but that repetitions are accumulated together into a single stronger memory trace. Repeating words in the context of different sentences might cause separate storage of the repetitions of a given word, because either word or sentence traces are stored separately. Separate storage would, in effect, convert a list-strength manipulation into a list-length manipulation and thereby induce a positive list-strength effect. In Experiment 1, this result was produced for single-word recognition and for two types of sentence recognition. In Experiment 2, both words and sentences were repeated together, which should have caused repetitions to be stored in a single, stronger, trace. As expected, the list-strength effect was eliminated. A sentence trace model was fit to the data, supporting the account of Shiffrin et al. (1990) and supporting an account of word and sentence recognition in which activation is summed for representations of all list items. The results from the two studies are inconsistent with most current models of memory (as shown by the theoretical analyses of Shiffrin et al., 1990) and pose an additional challenge for theory. 相似文献
862.
H G Smid W Lamain M M Hogeboom G Mulder L J Mulder 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1991,17(3):696-714
This study was designed to test whether information transmission between the perceptual and motor levels occurs continuously or in discrete steps. Ss performed visual search across nontargets that shared visual features with one of two possible targets, each assigned to a different response. In addition to reaction time, psychophysiological measures were used to assess the duration of target search and the onset of central and peripheral motor activity. Nontargets sharing features with a target selectively activated the response associated with that target, even when it was not present in the display. This suggests that information transmission to the motor level can consist of fine-grained visual information and that visual search and response selection occur in parallel. 相似文献
863.
Task dynamics and resource dynamics in the assembly of a coordinated rhythmic activity. 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
G P Bingham R C Schmidt M T Turvey L D Rosenblum 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1991,17(2):359-381
Task dynamics corresponding to rhythmic movements emerge from interactions among dynamical resources composed of the musculature, the link segments, and the nervous and circulatory systems. This article investigated whether perturbations of interlimb coordination might be effect over circulatory and nervous elements. Stiffness of wrist-pendulums oscillated at a common tempo and at 180 degrees relative phase was perturbed through the use of tonic activity about an ankle. Left and right stiffnesses, the common period, and the phase relation all changed. Stiffnesses increased with ankle torque in proportion to the wrist's inertial load. Despite different changes in stiffness at the two wrists, isochrony was preserved. The stability was shown to be consistent with the proportionality of changes in stiffness to the inertial loads. The phase departed from antiphase in proportion to the asymmetry of inertial loads. The size of departures decreased with increasing ankle torque. An account was developed in terms of muscular, circulatory, and nervous functions. 相似文献
864.
865.
Twenty reading comprehension-disabled (CD) and 20 reading comprehension and word recognition-disabled (CWRD), right-handed male children were matched with 20 normal-achieving age-matched controls and 20 normal-achieving reading level-matched controls and tested for left ear report on dichotic listening tasks using digits and consonant-vowel combinations (CVs). Left ear report for CVs and digits did not correlate for any of the groups. Both reading-disabled groups showed lower left ear report on digits. On CVs the CD group showed a high left ear report but only when there were no priming precursors, such as directions to attend right first and to process digits first. Priming effects interfered with the processing of both digits and CVs. Theoretically, the CWRD group seems to be characterized by a depressed right hemisphere, whereas the CD group may have a more labile right hemisphere, perhaps tending to overengagement for CV tasks but vulnerable to situational precursors in the form of priming effects. Implications extend to (1) subtyping practices in research with the learning-disabled, (2) inferences drawn from studies using different dichotic stimuli, and (3) the neuropsychology of reading disorders. 相似文献
866.
L Hochhaus K M Marohn 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1991,17(2):422-432
A visual prime succeeded by a brief target produces a paradox. Namely, target repetition yields poorer identification accuracy and shorter duration judgments than unrelated prime-target pairs. Experiment 1 manipulated stimulus onset asynchrony to learn when repetition blindness is maximized. Experiments 2 and 3 manipulated expectancy of repetitions through changes in the proportion of repeated trials and instructions, respectively. Results indicate that repetition blindness is influenced by subject strategies and that the change is not mediated by response bias. Experiment 4 showed that increasing the spatial distance between prime and target reduced but did not eliminate repetition blindness. The current data support joint explanation of repetition blindness in terms of perceptual capture (prime-target fusion) and token individuation failure (problems in encoding episodic reoccurrences of an event). 相似文献
867.
Albino Sprague-Dawley rats with complete septal lesions and rats with control operations were studied under fixed-ratio (FR) schedules of reinforcement. Both groups were trained for 10 sessions each under FR 10, 20, 40, 60, 80, and 100. In contrast to findings from progressive FR studies and some simple FR studies, septal lesions resulted in lower overall and local response rates along with longer postreinforcement pauses. These effects were especially evident during the FR 100 schedule of reinforcement. A comparison of reinforcement rate as a function of FR size within the context of behavioral economics (i.e., a demand function) indicated that septal lesions did not alter the reward value of food. These findings suggest that responding on FR schedules of reinforcement can be altered by the various procedures used to train rats to reach the terminal value of a reinforcement schedule. 相似文献
868.
Gender differences in health-related quality of life 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
In 1986 the life expectancy at birth was 71.3 years for males and 78.3 years for females--providing a 7-year advantage for women. Although women live longer, it has been reported that they paradoxically experience more physical and psychological illnesses. In this article, we estimate the expected well-years or quality-adjusted life years for men and women in the general population. The data were obtained in a random sample of 1,034 residents of San Diego. The well-life expectancy uses standard life expectancies with adjustments for quality of life. The well-life expectancy for men was 59.8 years; for women, it was 62.7 years. Thus, the quality adjustment had significantly more impact on women (15.6 years) than on men (11.5 years). Age-specific estimates of health-related quality of life suggested a male advantage before age 45 and a female advantage after age 45. The benefits of well-years of life as a public health statistic are discussed. 相似文献
869.
THE IMPACT OF ROLE STRESSORS AND PERSONAL RESOURCES ON THE STRESS EXPERIENCE OF PROFESSIONAL WOMEN 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
This study explored the contributions of role stressors and personal resources in predicting strain symptoms experienced by 117 professional women employed full-time in academia. Results of multiple regression analyses revealed that, while role stressors alone accounted for only a moderate amount of the variance in reported strain symptoms, 51 % of the variance was explained when both stressors and personal resources were combined. Womend, who experienced higher levels of personal control and social support as well as a greater number of roles occupied reported lower levels of strain symptoms. These results support the use of a transactional model of stress in future multiple role research. 相似文献
870.
The present study examined the differences in internal and external locus of control and imposter phenomenon among persons
with alcoholic and non-alcoholic parents. The subjects were 48 college students and 21 members of local Adult Children of
Alcoholic groups. The students were further divided into those with alcoholic parents and non-alcoholic parents. The subjects
were administered the following three instruments: the Nowicki-Strickland Internal-External Control Scale for Adults (Nowicki
& Duke, 1973), the Imposter Test (Chance & O’Toole, 1987), and a family history survey. Analysis of locus of control scores
revealed a highly significant (p<.01) difference for externality, with Adult Children being most external and students with
non-alcoholic parents least external. Similarly, internality scores differed significantly (p<.0001), with Adult Children
being least, and the non-alcoholic group being most internally oriented. Analysis of variance for imposter phenomenon revealed
a significant (p<.05) difference among the three groups, with the Adult Children of Alcoholics group having the highest scores
and students with non-alcoholic parents having the lowest. These findings are consistent with the idea that parental alcoholism
interferes with nurturing and consistent reinforcement, and with the development of personal adaptability. 相似文献