全文获取类型
收费全文 | 7696篇 |
免费 | 257篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
7958篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 81篇 |
2019年 | 92篇 |
2018年 | 137篇 |
2017年 | 140篇 |
2016年 | 164篇 |
2015年 | 125篇 |
2014年 | 126篇 |
2013年 | 769篇 |
2012年 | 267篇 |
2011年 | 276篇 |
2010年 | 181篇 |
2009年 | 216篇 |
2008年 | 280篇 |
2007年 | 313篇 |
2006年 | 280篇 |
2005年 | 256篇 |
2004年 | 244篇 |
2003年 | 299篇 |
2002年 | 267篇 |
2001年 | 108篇 |
2000年 | 106篇 |
1999年 | 131篇 |
1998年 | 102篇 |
1997年 | 115篇 |
1996年 | 110篇 |
1995年 | 125篇 |
1994年 | 96篇 |
1993年 | 116篇 |
1992年 | 101篇 |
1991年 | 92篇 |
1990年 | 77篇 |
1989年 | 79篇 |
1988年 | 84篇 |
1987年 | 97篇 |
1986年 | 80篇 |
1985年 | 90篇 |
1984年 | 114篇 |
1983年 | 119篇 |
1982年 | 107篇 |
1981年 | 102篇 |
1980年 | 109篇 |
1979年 | 107篇 |
1978年 | 115篇 |
1977年 | 107篇 |
1976年 | 119篇 |
1975年 | 92篇 |
1974年 | 94篇 |
1973年 | 89篇 |
1972年 | 58篇 |
1971年 | 45篇 |
排序方式: 共有7958条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
William E. Vandament Richard G. Burright Rollin R. Fessenden William H. Barker 《Behavior research methods》1970,2(6):290-296
Tables of sequences of two-class events are presented for use in programming psychological experiments in which behavior on trial n may be a function of the events of trials n ? 1, n ? 2, and/or n ? 3. Various factors related to schedule generation are discussed, i.e., restrictions on trial-block length which accompany sequential balance, interrelationships of trial blocks in the multiblock experiment, relationships between run length and r-tuple occurrences, and alternation behavior. Following a consideration of various methods of schedule generation for the two-class experiment, it was concluded that no method can result in schedules that possess all properties considered desirable in psychological experiments. However, the present sequences allow for sequential balance and analysis, and thus should prove useful in producing schedules in some contexts that are standard with regard to sequential influences. 相似文献
72.
M. B. Sterman J. J. Lynch M. T. Orne D. Paskewitz J. Costello N. Nicolov O. Diankov M. Popova E. Tsvetanska Robert G. Grenell J. J. Izquierdo V. H. Mark Joseph Germana N. Zill William P. Paré Joseph E. O. Newton George C. Offutt Walter Ehrlich Joseph E. D. Newton Julij Tosef J. Perez-Cruet Chester R. Wilpizeski John F. Lontz Andrew Livingston Joseph W. Cullen Samuel A. Corson Herman R. Weed Elizabeth O. Corson O. D. Murphree Paul N. Morgan Ruth Jarman J. Antal T. A. Ban J. V. Ananth H. E. Lehmann A. Ulric Moore Richard H. Barnes Wilson G. Pond 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1970,5(3):185-193
73.
Richard Schlegel 《Synthese》1970,21(1):65-82
The statistical aspects of quantum explanation are intrinsic to quantum physics; individual quantum events are created in the interactions associated with observation and are not describable by predictive theory. The superposition principle shows the essential difference between quantum and non-quantum physics, and the principle is exemplified in the classic single-photon two-slit interference experiment. Recently Mandel and Pfleegor have done an experiment somewhat similar to the optical single-photon experiment but with two independently operated lasers; interference is obtained even with beam intensity so small that only one photon is in the apparatus at a time. The result can be understood in terms of the superposition of states; or, in terms of the Uncertainty Principle, which is found to forbid the determination of which of the two lasers is the source of a given photon (if conditions for interference are to obtain). The Mandel-Pfleegor experiment gives a physical argument against the continuous localization of a photon that is assumed in the hidden variable theories and therefore gives further support for the generally accepted view that an observed entity (observed state) is created in the observation event. This aspect of quantum physics implies a subjectivism on the level of individual quantum-level occurrences, since there is in quantum theory no basis for asserting the existence of the event independently of observation of it. Extension of this subjectivism to large scale, non-quantum phenomena falls within the principles of quantum theory; counter considerations that argue against such an extension are noted. 相似文献
74.
Richard D. Gilson 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1969,5(3):181-182
Threshold elevations (TE) were determined at a testsite on the left thigh with the simultaneous presentation of combinations of maskers positioned on the trunk. It was found that the resultant TE from groups of up to 10 multiple maskers is nearly equal to the addition of the TE produced by each masker separately. 相似文献
75.
The study compares the extent to which naturally structured data and artificial, relatively random data (both with the same basic parameters) produce simple structure factors which are uniquely determined. Two examples of unstructured matrices were compared with the ball problem matrix. The results show that an oblique position of maximum hyperplane count in the structured data differs from that in the unstructured by reaching a significantly more unique position in terms of the exactitude with which it is re-discoverable when starting from different positions, and by reaching (at the maximum) a significantly higher hyperplane count. 相似文献
76.
Changeover delay and concurrent schedules: some effects on relative performance measures 总被引:16,自引:16,他引:0 下载免费PDF全文
Richard L. Shull Stanley S. Pliskoff 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1967,10(6):517-527
The pigeon and the rat partition total response output between both schedules of a concurrent variable-interval pair. The quantitative nature of a partition seems critically dependent on the relative rates with which the two schedules provide reinforcements for responding, in addition to the changeover delay. The manner in which the changeover delay controls the partition was studied by varying the duration of the changeover delay from 0 to 20 sec with each of two pairs of concurrent variable-interval schedules, viz., Conc VI 1.5-min VI 1.5-min and Conc VI 1-min VI 3-min. Rats served as the subjects and brain stimulation was employed as the reinforcer. When the schedules were Conc VI 1.5-min VI 1.5-min, relative response rate approximated 0.50 at all values of the changeover delay. When the schedules were Conc VI 1-min VI 3-min, relative response rate, computed with respect to the VI 1-min schedule, increased when the duration of the changeover delay increased. Changeover rate decreased when the duration of the changeover delay increased. The decrease was the same for both VI schedules of the Conc VI 1.5-min VI 1.5-min pair but was more rapid for the VI 3-min schedule of the Conc VI 1-min VI 3-min pair. 相似文献
77.
78.
79.
The minimal transformation to orthonormality 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Richard M. Johnson 《Psychometrika》1966,31(1):61-66
The solution is presented to the problem of finding that orthonormal matrix closest in the least-square sense to a given matrix of full rank. An application is shown to the problem in multiple regression analysis of determining the importance of each independent variable. 相似文献
80.
Richard Mowsesian Brian R. C. Heath John W. M. Rothney 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1966,45(3):238-242
The relationship between the occupational preferences of 147 superior students and their fathers' occupations was traced over the 4-year period that the students attended high schools in Wisconsin. Analysis of the data indicated that both male and female superior students tended to state vocational preferences at the professional level early in high school and to maintain this preference throughout. Their occupational preferences were generally at a higher level than those of their fathers. There was no trend away from general preferences for work at a professional level toward naming of specific occupations within that level. The results suggest that theories of vocational development that imply that stages are passed through during later adolescent periods do not apply to the superior student population of this study. 相似文献