首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   8660篇
  免费   133篇
  8793篇
  2020年   95篇
  2019年   112篇
  2018年   165篇
  2017年   162篇
  2016年   195篇
  2015年   148篇
  2014年   145篇
  2013年   844篇
  2012年   301篇
  2011年   320篇
  2010年   202篇
  2009年   244篇
  2008年   315篇
  2007年   340篇
  2006年   305篇
  2005年   273篇
  2004年   267篇
  2003年   310篇
  2002年   281篇
  2001年   124篇
  2000年   121篇
  1999年   143篇
  1998年   110篇
  1997年   126篇
  1996年   122篇
  1995年   128篇
  1994年   101篇
  1993年   123篇
  1992年   118篇
  1991年   106篇
  1990年   81篇
  1989年   94篇
  1988年   93篇
  1987年   103篇
  1986年   87篇
  1985年   104篇
  1984年   122篇
  1983年   124篇
  1982年   111篇
  1981年   112篇
  1980年   112篇
  1979年   110篇
  1978年   121篇
  1977年   111篇
  1976年   121篇
  1975年   98篇
  1974年   103篇
  1973年   92篇
  1972年   63篇
  1968年   52篇
排序方式: 共有8793条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
131.
The last decade has seen an increase in investigations of the psycholinguistic and production attributes of sign and the features of sign which influence learning and memory. However, these investigations usually have not adequately delineated between the learning of individual signs as opposed to the learning of sign lexicons or sign strings and have not discussed features which might differentially influence the learning and recall of single signs and sign lexicons. The current paper reviews existing literature in the area of sign learning and identifies psycholinguistic, psychological, and production features which may influence learning and recall of individual signs as well as sign lexicons. Included in the discussion of features which are hypothesized to influence the learning of individual signs are sign translucency (rather than sign iconicity) and referential concreteness of the sign gloss. Varibles hypothesized to influence sign lexicon learning are cheremic similarity, acoustical confusion, and their relationship to proactive and retroactive interference. Implications of the effects of these variables on the learning of individual signs and on the learning of sign lexicons are discussed.  相似文献   
132.
This paper reflects the working through of a group's fantasy that the deceased group leader wished the group to continue. As in any defense, the fantasy protected members from the pain of the finality of the leader's death. This article reveals the subjective experience concerning the effect of the leader's death on the group and members' intrapsychic struggle to separate and individuate from the deceased leader, while attending to and striving toward the group's continuation. The group's painful and growing individuation enabled the group to follow its own tasks without the spector of the deceased leader.  相似文献   
133.
134.
135.
It was hypothesized that one mechanism of self-control that children develop is the strategic capacity to select the experiences they encounter. This hypothesis led to the prediction that children would deal with certain aversive social experiences by seeking out or taking advantage of opportunities for nurturant experiences. Young children were exposed to an aversive social experience in which they received less nurturance than a peer, a positive experience in which they received more nurturance, or a neutral experience in which nurturance was equal. Subsequently, an opportunity was provided for children to control the length of time they watched a highly nurturant television program. As predicted, boys experiencing an aversive social encounter increased the length of time they exposed themselves to the nurturant television show, and their level of reduced positive affect was related to how long they watched the nurturant content, further supporting the interpretation that they did so in response to their own affective state. Girls did not adopt the strategy of self-exposure to nurturant television but did appear to engage in self-distraction during the aversive social experience. Despite the apparent use of control strategies, there was no indication that these strategies were effective for the amelioration of reduced positive affect resulting from the aversive social experience. Discussion focuses on the sex differences observed in the adoption of strategic behavior and factors contributing to the ineffectiveness of the control strategies. A general model is proposed for personal and environmental factors requisite for the selection, employment, and effectiveness of strategies to control experiences and their affective consequences.  相似文献   
136.
The hierarchical and radex models of ability organization are shown to be parallel. Both models suggest a complexity continuum along which cognitive performance tasks can be arrayed. In our revised radex model, the complexity continuum from the center to the periphery is shown to correspond to the general-to-specific dimension in factor analyses, or to test correlations with the general factor; complexity is redefined as apparent processing complexity. Examination of the theoretical and empirical bases for this continuum indicates its central importance for theories of intelligence.  相似文献   
137.
138.
139.
Word repetitions in sentence recognition   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
When some items on a list are strengthened by extra study time or repetitions, recognition of other, unrelated, list items is not harmed (Ratcliff, Clark, & Shiffrin, 1990). Shiffrin, Ratcliff, and Clark (1990) accounted for this list-strength finding with a model assuming that different items are stored separately in memory, but that repetitions are accumulated together into a single stronger memory trace. Repeating words in the context of different sentences might cause separate storage of the repetitions of a given word, because either word or sentence traces are stored separately. Separate storage would, in effect, convert a list-strength manipulation into a list-length manipulation and thereby induce a positive list-strength effect. In Experiment 1, this result was produced for single-word recognition and for two types of sentence recognition. In Experiment 2, both words and sentences were repeated together, which should have caused repetitions to be stored in a single, stronger, trace. As expected, the list-strength effect was eliminated. A sentence trace model was fit to the data, supporting the account of Shiffrin et al. (1990) and supporting an account of word and sentence recognition in which activation is summed for representations of all list items. The results from the two studies are inconsistent with most current models of memory (as shown by the theoretical analyses of Shiffrin et al., 1990) and pose an additional challenge for theory.  相似文献   
140.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号