全文获取类型
收费全文 | 7726篇 |
免费 | 258篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
7989篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 83篇 |
2019年 | 93篇 |
2018年 | 139篇 |
2017年 | 141篇 |
2016年 | 165篇 |
2015年 | 125篇 |
2014年 | 126篇 |
2013年 | 771篇 |
2012年 | 269篇 |
2011年 | 279篇 |
2010年 | 181篇 |
2009年 | 218篇 |
2008年 | 281篇 |
2007年 | 313篇 |
2006年 | 280篇 |
2005年 | 256篇 |
2004年 | 244篇 |
2003年 | 300篇 |
2002年 | 268篇 |
2001年 | 108篇 |
2000年 | 106篇 |
1999年 | 131篇 |
1998年 | 102篇 |
1997年 | 116篇 |
1996年 | 110篇 |
1995年 | 127篇 |
1994年 | 97篇 |
1993年 | 118篇 |
1992年 | 103篇 |
1991年 | 91篇 |
1990年 | 78篇 |
1989年 | 80篇 |
1988年 | 84篇 |
1987年 | 97篇 |
1986年 | 81篇 |
1985年 | 90篇 |
1984年 | 114篇 |
1983年 | 119篇 |
1982年 | 107篇 |
1981年 | 102篇 |
1980年 | 109篇 |
1979年 | 108篇 |
1978年 | 115篇 |
1977年 | 107篇 |
1976年 | 119篇 |
1975年 | 91篇 |
1974年 | 94篇 |
1973年 | 88篇 |
1972年 | 58篇 |
1971年 | 45篇 |
排序方式: 共有7989条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
61.
The idealized physique preferences of a group of 94 male college students were measured and correlated with scales measuring attitudes toward women and personality rigidity. Three hypotheses, derived from sex-role research, were tested and confirmed: (1) Males who have negative attitudes toward women would like most to have a tapering V physique. (2) Males who have a rigid personality structure have stronger preference for an idealized muscular physique than males with more flexible personalities. (3) Males have a generally greater preference for a tapering V physique than women. It was concluded, from the results of this and other studies, that body preference is related to certain aspects of sex-role attitudes. 相似文献
62.
Richard A. Griggs 《Current Psychology》1984,3(4):3-10
The present study examined the hypothesis that violation and role-playing instructions are necessary conditions for facilitation
on Wason’s selection task and that facilitation due to memory cueing via thematic content is secondary. The results of a factorial
experiment employing these factors did not support the hypothesis. Memory cueing was indicated as the primary factor, but
violation instructions did increase the amount of facilitation observed. This interactive effect and the nature of facilitation
by memory cueing are briefly discussed. 相似文献
63.
64.
Richard Routley 《Studia Logica》1984,43(1-2):131-158
American-plan semantics with 4 values 1, 0, { {1, 0}} {{}}, interpretable as True, False, Both and Neither, are furnished for a range of logics, including relevant affixing systems. The evaluation rules for extensional connectives take a classical form: in particular, those for negation assume the form 1 (A, a) iff 0 (A, a) and 0 (A, a) iff 1 (A, a), so eliminating the star function *, on which much criticism of relevant logic semantics has focussed. The cost of these classical features is a further relation (or operation), required in evaluating falsity assignments of implication formulae.Two styles of 4 valued relational semantics are developed; firstly a semantics using notions of double truth and double validity for basic relevant systemB and some extensions of it; and secondly, since the first semantics makes heavy weather of validating negation principles such as Contraposition, a reduced semantics using more complex implicational rules for relevant systemC and various of its extensions. To deal satisfactorily with elite systemsR,E andT, however, further complication is inevitable; and a relation of mateship (suggested by the Australian plan) is introduced to permit cross-over from 1 to 0 values and vice versa. 相似文献
65.
In IV, xvii, 19–22 of his Essay, Locke employs Latin labels for four kinds of argument, of which one (ad hominem) was already in circulation and one (ad judicium) has never had much currency. The present proposal seeks to locate and clarify what Locke was aiming to describe, and to contrast what he says with some subsequent uses that have been made of these labels as if they named fallacies. Though three of the four kinds of argument that Locke picks out are often less than decisive, he casts no aspersion on the legitimacy of their use in debate.
相似文献66.
67.
68.
69.
70.
Two experiments were run to investigate the locus of selectivity in a visual memory task involving differential payoff for letters correctly recalled: Major variables consisted of value instructions given before or after the exposure of a display, the ratio of high to low value, and exposure duration. The results showed that Ss make more correct reports of higher value than of lesser value, whether value instructions are given before or after the display. Furthermore, overall performance and extent of selectivity increase with increasing exposure time, but more for the before than the after conditions of instruction. These results suggest that selectivity occurs both during perception and retention and during recall. 相似文献