全文获取类型
收费全文 | 39199篇 |
免费 | 725篇 |
国内免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
39930篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 421篇 |
2019年 | 522篇 |
2018年 | 716篇 |
2017年 | 696篇 |
2016年 | 808篇 |
2015年 | 546篇 |
2014年 | 648篇 |
2013年 | 3122篇 |
2012年 | 1245篇 |
2011年 | 1252篇 |
2010年 | 768篇 |
2009年 | 863篇 |
2008年 | 1095篇 |
2007年 | 1145篇 |
2006年 | 1008篇 |
2005年 | 892篇 |
2004年 | 868篇 |
2003年 | 860篇 |
2002年 | 859篇 |
2001年 | 1151篇 |
2000年 | 1146篇 |
1999年 | 893篇 |
1998年 | 422篇 |
1997年 | 410篇 |
1996年 | 394篇 |
1995年 | 421篇 |
1993年 | 398篇 |
1992年 | 719篇 |
1991年 | 675篇 |
1990年 | 635篇 |
1989年 | 619篇 |
1988年 | 615篇 |
1987年 | 566篇 |
1986年 | 553篇 |
1985年 | 612篇 |
1984年 | 536篇 |
1983年 | 511篇 |
1982年 | 408篇 |
1979年 | 575篇 |
1978年 | 432篇 |
1977年 | 402篇 |
1976年 | 420篇 |
1975年 | 440篇 |
1974年 | 522篇 |
1973年 | 538篇 |
1972年 | 452篇 |
1971年 | 384篇 |
1968年 | 459篇 |
1967年 | 401篇 |
1966年 | 378篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
961.
Gutchess AH Welsh RC Boduroglu A Park DC 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2006,6(2):102-109
Behavioral research suggests that Westerners focus more on objects, whereas East Asians attend more to relationships and contexts.
We evaluated the neural basis for these cultural differences in an event-related fMRI study. East Asian and American participants
incidentally encoded pictures of (1) a target object alone, (2) a background scene with no discernable target object, and
(3) a distinct target object against a meaningful background. Americans, relative to East Asians, activated more regions implicated
in object processing, including bilateral middle temporal gyrus, left superior parietal/angular gyrus, and right superior
temporal/supramarginal gyrus. In contrast to the cultural differences in object-processing areas, few differences emerged
in background-processing regions. These results suggest that cultural experiences subtly direct neural activity, particularly
for focal objects, at an early stage of scene encoding. 相似文献
962.
Maureen C. McHugh 《Sex roles》2006,54(5-6):361-369
Recent attempts to medicalize women’s sexual “dysfunction” are critiqued and a “New View” of women’s sexual problems is introduced. The author argues for a female-centered perspective on women’s sexual desires and problems, based on a review of the literature on women’s sexuality and her observations of young women’s sexual experiences from 25 years of teaching Human Sexuality to undergraduate women. The review suggests that a pill or a patch cannot adequately address the sexual problems commonly experienced by US women. 相似文献
963.
Rape myth acceptance has been extensively studied. Little research is available, however, on the relationship of this variable
to other oppressive belief systems. A sample of 492 male and 506 female college students completed the Illinois Rape Myth
Acceptance Scale, the Attitudes Toward Women Scale (short form), the Neosexism Scale, the Modern and Old Fashioned Racism
Scale, the Modern Homophobia Scale, a modified version of the Economic Belief Scale, the Fraboni Scale of Ageism, and the
Marlowe–Crowne Social Desirability Scale (short form). Because there were no existing measures of intolerance toward multiple
religions, the Religious Intolerance Scale was developed for this study (using relevant items from the Godfrey Richman Isms
Scale). Findings here suggested that greater racism (both modern and old fashioned), sexism (both modern and old fashioned),
homophobia (toward both gay men and lesbians), ageism, classism, and religious intolerance were each associated with greater
rape myth acceptance. Moreover, each belief system collectively added to the prediction of rape myth acceptance, although
sexism has the highest overlap with rape myth acceptance. Although gender did not moderate the relationship between oppressive
belief systems and rape myth acceptance, results, across analyses, did indicate that men reported greater rape myth acceptance
than women did. Results point to the interrelatedness of rape myth acceptance, racism, sexism, homophobia, classism, ageism,
and religious intolerance. 相似文献
964.
Stams GJ Brugman D Deković M van Rosmalen L van der Laan P Gibbs JC 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2006,34(5):692-708
A meta-analysis of 50 studies was conducted to investigate whether juvenile delinquents use lower levels of moral judgment than their nondelinquent age-mates and, if so, what factors may influence or moderate the developmental delay. The results show a lower stage of moral judgment for juvenile delinquents (d=.76). Effect sizes were large for comparisons involving male offenders, late adolescents, delinquents with low intelligence, and incarcerated delinquents. The largest effect sizes were found for period of incarceration and comparisons involving juvenile delinquents with psychopathic disorder. Production instead of recognition measures, dilemma-free assessment methods, and non-blind scoring procedures yielded relatively large effect sizes, whereas effect sizes were medium for comparisons involving delinquents with average intelligence, non-incarcerated delinquents, female offenders, as well as early and middle adolescents. Psychopathic disorder and institutionalization were identified as unique moderators of the link between moral judgment and juvenile delinquency. It is concluded that developmentally delayed moral judgment is strongly associated with juvenile delinquency, even after controlling for socioeconomic status, gender, age and intelligence.This article stems from a paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Moral Education, July 2006, Fribourg, Switzerland. 相似文献
965.
966.
967.
Thewissen R Snijders SJ Havermans RC van den Hout M Jansen A 《Behaviour research and therapy》2006,44(10):1441-1449
The effects of cue exposure therapy are limited, because renewal after extinction is an important source of relapse. In this study, 33 smokers were exposed to a cue predicting smoking availability and a cue predicting smoking unavailability in one context (acquisition context A). Following extinction in another context (extinction context B), a test for renewal took place in the original acquisition context A (i.e. ABA renewal). Urge to smoke was measured using a Visual Analogue Scale. Renewal of differential conditioned urge responding occurred when participants were tested in the acquisition context, while differential urge responding remained extinguished when tested in the extinction context. This experiment provides evidence that ABA renewal occurred in smokers. Clinical implications are discussed. 相似文献
968.
Creel SC Tanenhaus MK Aslin RN 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2006,32(1):15-32
Four experiments examined effects of lexical stress on lexical access for recently learned words. Participants learned artificial lexicons (48 words) containing phonologically similar items and were tested on their knowledge in a 4-alternative forced-choice (4AFC) referent-selection task. Lexical stress differences did not reduce confusions between cohort items: KAdazu and kaDAzeI were confused with one another in a 4AFC task and in gaze fixations as often as BOsapeI and BOsapaI. However, lexical stress did affect the relative likelihood of stress-initial confusions when words were embedded in running nonsense speech. Words with medial stress, regardless of initial vowel quality, were more prone to confusions than words with initial stress. The authors concluded that non-initial stress, particularly when wor segmentation is difficult, may serve as "noise" that alters lexical learning and lexical access. 相似文献
969.
In three experiments, we investigated the structure of frontoparallel haptic space. In the first experiment, we asked blindfolded participants to rotate a matching bar so that it felt parallel to the reference bar, the bars could be at various positions in the frontoparallel plane. Large systematic errors were observed, in which orientations that were perceived to be parallel were not physically parallel. In two subsequent experiments, we investigated the origin of these errors. In Experiment 2, we asked participants to verbally report the orientation of haptically presented bars. In this task, participants made errors that were considerably smaller than those made in Experiment 1. In Experiment 3, we asked participants to set bars in a verbally instructed orientation, and they also made errors significantly smaller than those observed in Experiment 1. The data suggest that the errors in the matching task originate from the transfer of the reference orientation to the matching-bar position. 相似文献
970.
The personality construct of hardiness, III: Relationships with repression, innovativeness, authoritarianism, and performance 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Previous research has established hardiness as a dispositional factor in preserving and enhancing performance and health despite stressful circumstances. The present four studies continue this construct-validational process by (a) introducing a shortened version of the hardiness measure and (b) testing hypotheses concerning the relationship between hardiness and repressive coping, right-wing authoritarianism, innovative behavior, and billable hours (a measure of consulting effectiveness). Results of these studies suggest the adequate reliability and validity of the Personal Views Survey III-R, which is the shortened, 18-item measure of hardiness. Further, results support the hypothesis that the relationship of hardiness is negative with repressive coping and right-wing authoritarianism and positive with innovative behavior and billable hours. Hardiness also appears unrelated to socially desirable responding. 相似文献