全文获取类型
收费全文 | 7962篇 |
免费 | 124篇 |
专业分类
8086篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 82篇 |
2019年 | 96篇 |
2018年 | 137篇 |
2017年 | 144篇 |
2016年 | 166篇 |
2015年 | 129篇 |
2014年 | 133篇 |
2013年 | 774篇 |
2012年 | 274篇 |
2011年 | 283篇 |
2010年 | 185篇 |
2009年 | 217篇 |
2008年 | 282篇 |
2007年 | 315篇 |
2006年 | 284篇 |
2005年 | 257篇 |
2004年 | 246篇 |
2003年 | 299篇 |
2002年 | 267篇 |
2001年 | 109篇 |
2000年 | 107篇 |
1999年 | 131篇 |
1998年 | 104篇 |
1997年 | 117篇 |
1996年 | 116篇 |
1995年 | 125篇 |
1994年 | 97篇 |
1993年 | 119篇 |
1992年 | 104篇 |
1991年 | 97篇 |
1990年 | 77篇 |
1989年 | 80篇 |
1988年 | 84篇 |
1987年 | 98篇 |
1986年 | 80篇 |
1985年 | 92篇 |
1984年 | 114篇 |
1983年 | 120篇 |
1982年 | 109篇 |
1981年 | 106篇 |
1980年 | 109篇 |
1979年 | 107篇 |
1978年 | 117篇 |
1977年 | 109篇 |
1976年 | 119篇 |
1975年 | 92篇 |
1974年 | 96篇 |
1973年 | 90篇 |
1972年 | 59篇 |
1971年 | 47篇 |
排序方式: 共有8086条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
161.
162.
Richard F. Dillon 《Behavior research methods》1979,11(2):234-240
Seven basic forms of structured flow of control, sequential execution, IF-THEN-ELSE, DO-WHILE, REPEAT-UNTIL, SELECT-CASE, ITERATIVE-DO, and LOOP-EXITIF-ENDLOOP, are described. The application of the forms is illustrated with examples of typical problems from the psychology laboratory. The feasibility of implementing the basic forms is demonstrated by showing how they have been incorporated in a high-level language for controlling human learning, memory, and perception experiments. 相似文献
163.
164.
165.
Auditory intensity in the conditioning treatment of enuresis nocturna was examined in a study by Young and Morgan (1973a). In their study of three alarm intensities, no significant differences either in terms of treatment efficacy or of subsequent relapse were found. For both theoretical and practical reasons, the finding of no differences in treatment parameters as a function of varying alarm intensities is puzzling. In learning theory terms, a very intense UCS (bell or buzzer) would be expected to lead to more rapid acquisition of dryness (conditioned response) than a less intense UCS. Learning theory notwithstanding, the depth of somnolence of the enuretic child is legend. Not only are testimonials from parents regarding difficulty in arousal of their bedwetting child commonly elicited, but the literature on causes and treatment of bedwetting is replete with evidence of a deep-sleep enuresis syndrome (Bostock, 1962). It has been demonstrated by Finley (1971) and Di Perri and Meduri (1972) that the arousal threshold of enuretic children is abnormally high compared to nonenuretic children. The failure of the enuretic to awaken to the presentation of a bell (UCS) is frequently cited as responsible for unsuccessful bell-and-pad conditioning (Browning, 1967). Young and Morgan (1973b) in their study on rapidity of response to the conditioning treatment for enuresis found that one treatment problem most significantly related to slow responders was failure of the alarm stimulus to awaken the child. For those unfamiliar with the literature, the Young and Morgan (1973b) finding appears particularly surprising when one considers that they employed as their alarm UCS “...a powerful auditory stimulus...” (p. 490).Because of these important theoretical as well as practical concerns, an attempt was made to examine the effect of two auditory intensities on treatment and relapse parameters in the conditioning of enuresis nocturna. 相似文献
166.
Richard N Aslin 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1977,23(1):133-150
Two experiments measured changes in binocular eye alignment from 1- to 6-month-old human infants. In Experiment 1 changes in binocular eye alignment were recorded from 1, 2, and 3 month olds using corneal photography. A luminous target was moved along the infant's midline at one of two constant speeds. Infants at all three ages showed some evidence of appropriate changes in binocular eye alignment (convergence as the target approached and divergence as the target receded). The likelihood of appropriate convergence and divergence increased with age, as did the ability to respond appropriately to the faster target motion. A measure of convergence lag (further decrease in interpupillary distance after target motion had ended) was obtained for trials on which the target approached the infant. Convergence lag decreased with age and was greater on trials at the faster target speed until 3 months of age. Experiment 2 measured the responses of 3, , and 6 month olds as a wedge prism was placed alternately in front of each eye. The prism displaced the image nasally (either 2.5° or 5°), creating inappropriate binocular eye alignment. A refixation eye movement in response to the prism was not present consistently until 6 months of age. Hypotheses as to the mechanism controlling infant binocular eye alignment are discussed with respect both to the present findings and to findings from human adults and animals. 相似文献
167.
Sharon S. Koenigs Martha L. Fiedler Richard Decharms 《Journal of applied social psychology》1977,7(2):95-114
The study reports relationships between teachers' belief systems (Harvey), a teacher-pupil interaction measure of classroom influence, pupils' perception of the dimate of the dassroom, and pupils' academic achievement. Results demonstrate that more complex teachers encourage more pupil influence, and are perceived as having a more origin climate. In classrooms of high pupil influence and perceived origin climate, the pupils have higher academic achievement. These results are discussed in terms of the theory of personal causation and are cited as evidence for the value of the study of action sequences as related to verbal-utterances of the actors. 相似文献
168.
Dr. Richard D. Kahoe Ph.D. 《Pastoral Psychology》1977,25(3):197-207
The concepts of task-centered (intrinsic) and self-centered (extrinsic) religion have been found useful in a psychological understanding of Christianity. The task-centered dimension is related to a healthy, mature religion, and the self-centered dimension is related to much that has been found to be unhealthy about religion. Theologically the former concept implies a turning to God and His work and away from self. Implications are discussed in terms of the pastor as a model of a task-centered and not a self-centered Christian. In the pastor's role of promoting task-centered religion, special attention is given to worship and to implications for ministering to people's hurts and troubles. 相似文献
169.
Expectancy and value scores from 376 engineers were used to develop a taxonomy of nine psychological categories, these being differentially related to motivation and satisfaction. Static and longitudinal analyses (over 4 years) revealed significant relationships between age, prior category, and organizational reward practices, on the one hand, and the distribution and movement of respondents across categories, on the other. Results suggested a dynamic, three-stage psychological model of careers in engineering. 相似文献
170.
Richard Chechile 《Memory & cognition》1977,5(5):535-540
A statistical procedure that separates storage from retrieval was used to study the acoustic similarity effect as a function of retention interval in the Brown-Peterson paradigm. Both storage and retrieval components showed reliable and independent changes with retention interval, but only the storage component was affected by acoustic similarity. Hence, acoustic similarity affects trace durability and retrieval plays no essential role in the similarity effect. This finding is inconsistent with the address hypothesis (cf. Baddeley, 1968). It is argued that acoustic similarity induces subjects to encode the target item in a confused fashion, particularly in regard to order information. 相似文献