首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   61415篇
  免费   2476篇
  国内免费   33篇
  2020年   716篇
  2019年   909篇
  2018年   1295篇
  2017年   1306篇
  2016年   1393篇
  2015年   944篇
  2014年   1126篇
  2013年   5509篇
  2012年   2037篇
  2011年   2208篇
  2010年   1307篇
  2009年   1360篇
  2008年   1995篇
  2007年   1983篇
  2006年   1756篇
  2005年   1576篇
  2004年   1544篇
  2003年   1483篇
  2002年   1480篇
  2001年   1896篇
  2000年   1800篇
  1999年   1422篇
  1998年   780篇
  1997年   694篇
  1996年   622篇
  1995年   636篇
  1994年   587篇
  1993年   609篇
  1992年   1111篇
  1991年   1044篇
  1990年   1039篇
  1989年   944篇
  1988年   913篇
  1987年   891篇
  1986年   889篇
  1985年   947篇
  1984年   771篇
  1983年   685篇
  1982年   560篇
  1981年   519篇
  1980年   523篇
  1979年   758篇
  1978年   589篇
  1976年   550篇
  1975年   669篇
  1974年   709篇
  1973年   675篇
  1972年   602篇
  1968年   582篇
  1967年   576篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
121.
122.
This research examines how maximizers make decisions when they must trade-off between desirability and feasibility attributes. Across four studies, we demonstrate that maximizers tend to prefer choices offering more desirability to those offering more feasibility and respond more favorably to a product's advertising when it highlights desirability more than feasibility attributes. Furthermore, we show that maximizers' focus on outcomes rather than processes drives their preference for desirability, such that changing from an outcome to a process focus can redirect their interest from desirability to feasibility. By contrast, satisficers do not prefer products higher in desirability to those higher in feasibility and are not more receptive to ads highlighting desirability attributes. Furthermore, because satisficers may focus on both the outcome and the process, priming either one is redundant and does not alter their preference for desirability or feasibility.  相似文献   
123.
It was predicted that trained observers would detect deception more accurately than untrained observers. More specifically, it was predicted that the highest deception detection accuracy would be found among trained observers judging the veracity of low self-monitors and unrehearsed liars, whereas the lowest detection accuracy would be found among untrained observers judging the veracity of high self-monitors and rehearsed deceivers. It also was hypothesized that the discrepancy between observers‘actual ability to detect deception and their certainty in the accuracy of their judgments would be smaller for trained observers than for untrained observers. Observers trained to detect deception used six behavioral cues based on research by deTurck and Miller (1985): (a) message duration, (b) response latency, (c) adaptors, (d) pauses, (e) nonfluencies, and (f) hand gestures. Results confirmed both hypotheses.  相似文献   
124.
Suicide and homicide rates by age were analyzed for Canada and the United States, indicating that suicide is higher in Canada and that homicide is higher in the United States. Results indicated a positive association between homicide and suicide rates in the United States but these two rates of death were not significantly associated in Canada. Holinger (1987) had associated the relative size of the cohort to the rates of suicide and homicide in young people in the United States. The more current data in both countries did not support Holinger's results. Using the measure devised by Easterlin (1980) and Ahlburg and Schapiro (1984) –that is, the proportion of youths aged 15–24 relative to adults aged 25–64–the correlation between the size of the youth cohort and the suicide rate of youths aged 15–24 was negative. It is concluded that the two patterns in these two countries may be explained from a historical perspective.  相似文献   
125.
We examined whether four personality traits—trait aggression, trait industriousness, trait deceptiveness, and trait self-control—were differentially related to 11 narrow-bandwidth CWB facets: (a) property destruction, (b) inappropriate verbal actions, (c) inappropriate physical actions, (d) poor attendance, (e) poor quality work, (f) unsafe behavior, (g) theft and related behavior, (h) misuse of information, (i) misuse of time and resources, (j) alcohol use, and (k) drug use. Based on responses from 404 employed participants recruited using Mechanical Turk, we found that each narrow-bandwidth personality trait often yielded stronger relationships with overall CWB than with narrow-bandwidth CWB facets.  相似文献   
126.
127.
128.
129.
130.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号