首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   30982篇
  免费   408篇
  国内免费   7篇
  2020年   127篇
  2019年   163篇
  2018年   3622篇
  2017年   2985篇
  2016年   2459篇
  2015年   376篇
  2014年   285篇
  2013年   1210篇
  2012年   923篇
  2011年   2737篇
  2010年   2726篇
  2009年   1740篇
  2008年   2060篇
  2007年   2520篇
  2006年   408篇
  2005年   557篇
  2004年   481篇
  2003年   492篇
  2002年   425篇
  2001年   231篇
  2000年   238篇
  1999年   209篇
  1998年   160篇
  1997年   166篇
  1996年   156篇
  1995年   165篇
  1994年   129篇
  1993年   141篇
  1992年   147篇
  1991年   142篇
  1990年   130篇
  1989年   127篇
  1988年   120篇
  1987年   134篇
  1986年   125篇
  1985年   133篇
  1984年   155篇
  1983年   151篇
  1982年   147篇
  1981年   136篇
  1980年   135篇
  1979年   145篇
  1978年   146篇
  1977年   132篇
  1976年   147篇
  1975年   126篇
  1974年   128篇
  1973年   116篇
  1972年   88篇
  1970年   79篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
11.
12.
13.
14.
15.
16.
17.
18.
19.
Previous research suggests that children gradually understand the mitigating effects of apology on damage to a transgressor's reputation. However, little is known about young children's insights into the central emotional implications of apology. In two studies, children ages 4–9 heard stories about moral transgressions in which the wrongdoers either did or did not apologize. In Study 1, children in the no‐apology condition showed the classic pattern of ‘happy victimizer’ attributions by expecting the wrongdoer to feel good about gains won via transgression. By contrast, in the apology condition, children attributed negative feelings to the transgressor and improved feelings to the victim. In Study 2, these effects were found even when the explicit emotion marker ‘sorry’ was removed from the apology exchange. Thus, young children understand some important emotional functions of apology.  相似文献   
20.
Even though it can be shown that verbal knowledge of results (KR) is redundant with sensory feedback for learning certain motor skills, such findings do not eliminate the possibility that when KR is available it influences underlying learning processes. In order to examine the function of KR more closely, two experiments were designed in which the subjects received conflicting information about their own sensory feedback and the KR presented by the experimenter. In Experiment 1, two erroneous-KR groups, a correct-KR group, and a no-KR group performed 150 practice trials on a simple anticipation timing task and then performed three no-KR retention tests of 30 trials each following intervals of 10 minutes, 1 week, and 1 month. The results supported previous findings that providing correct KR is redundant in anticipation tasks. However, learning was influenced by KR as subjects performed according to the erroneous KR information, thereby ignoring their sensory feedback even after a 1-month interval. In Experiment 2, subjects practised a more complex striking response for the anticipation task for 75 trials and then performed no-KR retention trials either immediately, or 1 day or 1 week later. One of the groups received erroneous KR after 50 practice trials with correct KR. The results confirmed and extended those from Experiment 1, as erroneous KR, even after initial practice with correct KR, influenced retention performance. These results indicate that although KR provides information that is not needed to learn anticipation timing skills, this augmented verbal information is a dominant source of information that influences underlying cognitive processes involved in learning motor skills.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号