全文获取类型
收费全文 | 28052篇 |
免费 | 130篇 |
专业分类
28182篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 84篇 |
2019年 | 98篇 |
2018年 | 3568篇 |
2017年 | 2922篇 |
2016年 | 2383篇 |
2015年 | 314篇 |
2014年 | 205篇 |
2013年 | 844篇 |
2012年 | 823篇 |
2011年 | 2626篇 |
2010年 | 2657篇 |
2009年 | 1670篇 |
2008年 | 1953篇 |
2007年 | 2427篇 |
2006年 | 306篇 |
2005年 | 462篇 |
2004年 | 407篇 |
2003年 | 403篇 |
2002年 | 326篇 |
2001年 | 138篇 |
2000年 | 158篇 |
1999年 | 149篇 |
1998年 | 125篇 |
1997年 | 137篇 |
1996年 | 125篇 |
1995年 | 125篇 |
1994年 | 99篇 |
1993年 | 120篇 |
1992年 | 106篇 |
1991年 | 97篇 |
1990年 | 86篇 |
1989年 | 79篇 |
1988年 | 86篇 |
1987年 | 98篇 |
1986年 | 82篇 |
1985年 | 91篇 |
1984年 | 114篇 |
1983年 | 120篇 |
1982年 | 110篇 |
1981年 | 106篇 |
1980年 | 113篇 |
1979年 | 107篇 |
1978年 | 118篇 |
1977年 | 111篇 |
1976年 | 123篇 |
1975年 | 92篇 |
1974年 | 96篇 |
1973年 | 91篇 |
1972年 | 59篇 |
1971年 | 47篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
221.
Huber, Shriffrin, Lyle, and Ruys (2001) measured short-term repetition priming effects in perceptual identification with two-alternative forced-choice testing. There was a preference to choose repeated words following passive viewing of primes and a preference against choosing repeated words following active responding to primes. In this present study, we explored conditions of prime processing that produce this pattern of results. Experiment 1 revealed that increased prime duration under passive viewing instructions produces the active priming pattern. Experiment 2 assessed memory for primes: With poor recognition of primes, there was a strong preference for repeated words; however, with good recognition of primes, this preference was eliminated. These results are modeled by a computational theory of optimal decision making, responding optimally with unknown sources of evidence (ROUSE). In ROUSE, a preference for repeated words results from source confusion between primes and choice words. A reversal in the direction of preference arises from the discounting of words known to have also appeared as primes. 相似文献
222.
The maximal passive ankle dorsiflexion angle and the maximal passive resistive torque at this angle were measured for 81 women 20 to 84 years of age and correlated with the passive-elastic stiffness (stiffness) of an ankle dorsiflexion stretch. Pearson correlation coefficients and multiple regression analyses were used to examine whether the two clinical measurements could predict ankle stiffness. The maximal passive resistive torque showed a moderate correlation with stiffness in the full stretch range (r = .69) and high correlation with stiffness in the last half of the full stretch range (r = .84). The maximal dorsiflexion angle showed a low correlation with stiffness in the full stretch range (r = .27) and in the last half of the full stretch range (r = .36). The maximal passive resistive torque and the dorsiflexion angle together accounted for 54% of the stiffness variance in the full stretch range and 76% of the stiffness variance in the last half of the full stretch range. Thus, the clinical measurements of the maximal passive dorsiflexion angle and the maximal passive resistive torque were directly and significantly related to the ankle dorsiflexion passive-elastic stiffness and good predictors of stiffness in the last half of the passive ankle dorsiflexion stretch. 相似文献
223.
Richard A. Chechile Lara N. Sloboda Jessica R. Chamberland 《Journal of mathematical psychology》2012,56(1):35-53
In this paper a new model, the Implicit/Explicit Separation (IES) model, is developed and applied. The model is designed to obtain separate probability measures for explicit memory storage, implicit memory storage, fractional storage, and non-storage. The model is needed because memory research has shown the importance of the distinction between a conscious memory of a target event and a memory residue that is unable to support conscious recall or confident recognition, but it is still able to support guessing at rates above chance. Maximum likelihood and population-parameter mapping estimates for the parameters of the IES model are provided. The accuracy of parameter estimates is studied as a function of sample size. Three experiments are reported to demonstrate how the IES model is used to achieve a more fine grained assessment of the quality of information storage. These experiments also provide strong validation support for the IES model itself. Across the three experiments, each of the four components for representing target information (explicit, implicit, fractional, and non-storage) demonstrates a different pattern. The IES model is discussed in terms of alternative models such as the dual-process model and the process-dissociation model. 相似文献
224.
225.
Peter J. Bieling Karen Rowa Martin M. Antony Laura J. Summerfeldt Richard P. Swinson 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2001,23(4):223-230
The Illness Intrusiveness Rating Scale (IIRS) is a measure designed to assess the impact of illness on various domains of functioning (G. M. Devins, 1994). In anxiety disordered patients, illness intrusiveness ratings are higher than those of chronically ill medical patients, suggesting that the IIRS may have a different underlying structure in a sample of individuals with anxiety disorders. To examine this possibility, IIRS items were submitted to an exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis in 2 samples (total N = 294). These solutions were compared to solutions in chronically ill populations from a previous study. In the exploratory analysis, both a one-factor and a three-factor solution were identified, accounting for 42 and 61% of the variance, respectively. Confirmatory analyses showed adequate similarity between the three-factor structure of the IIRS from a medically ill population and the current three-factor structure, suggesting that elevated IIRS scores in anxiety disordered samples cannot be explained simply by a different structure of the instrument. 相似文献
226.
227.
228.
Richard Coughlan Terry Connolly 《Organizational behavior and human decision processes》2001,85(2):1069
In decisions under uncertainty, decision makers confront two uncertainties: the uncertain linkage between actions and outcomes and the uncertain linkage between these outcomes and his or her affective responses to them. The two studies reported here examine affective responses to expected and unexpected outcomes in various settings. In Study 1, a scenario-based laboratory experiment (N = 149), we examined subjects' predicted responses to a range of outcomes, as a function of how surprising the outcome was. Study 2, a field study (N = 127), involved the expectations of bowlers about their scores in an upcoming game and about their responses to various outcomes at, above, and below expectations. We also measured actual affective reactions after the bowlers had completed their games. Findings suggest that subjects both expect and experience a loss-averse, expectation-based value function broadly of the Prospect Theory type. They also anticipate, and experience, an amplifying effect of outcome surprise, though they underestimate its size. We argue that such underestimation, together with overtight prediction ranges, may expose subjects to much larger affective variation with outcome variability than they anticipate. 相似文献
229.
In his classic sociological analysis The Power Elite, published in 1956, sociologist C. Wright Mills depicted a group of white Anglo-Saxon Protestant men who ran the corporate, political and military elites in the United States. This article looks at this power elite 40 years later to consider the extent to which diversity has occurred. Each of these three institutions (the corporate, the political, and the military) is examined to see if jews, women, African-Americans, Latinos, Asian Americans, and homosexuals have penetrated into the highest circles. The patterns that appear across all categories of newcomers are discussed, as is the extent to which the arrival of some newcomers has led to changes in the behavior of the power elite. 相似文献
230.