首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   105篇
  免费   12篇
  2024年   3篇
  2023年   2篇
  2022年   2篇
  2021年   1篇
  2020年   4篇
  2019年   4篇
  2018年   6篇
  2017年   2篇
  2016年   1篇
  2015年   4篇
  2014年   1篇
  2013年   14篇
  2012年   4篇
  2011年   7篇
  2010年   1篇
  2009年   4篇
  2008年   6篇
  2007年   5篇
  2006年   2篇
  2005年   1篇
  2004年   3篇
  2003年   3篇
  2002年   5篇
  2001年   1篇
  2000年   2篇
  1999年   3篇
  1998年   1篇
  1996年   2篇
  1994年   2篇
  1992年   1篇
  1991年   1篇
  1989年   1篇
  1985年   1篇
  1983年   1篇
  1980年   1篇
  1979年   3篇
  1978年   1篇
  1976年   2篇
  1975年   2篇
  1974年   1篇
  1968年   2篇
  1967年   2篇
  1966年   1篇
  1962年   1篇
排序方式: 共有117条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
ABSTRACT

One of the more remarkable trends of the past 30 years is the dramatic rise of individuals who do not identify with any religious tradition. While this trend has been well documented, some of the underlying dynamics and consequences have not been fully appreciated or explicated. We examine the General Social Survey in the period from 1972 to 2014 to examine how the increase in the ‘nones’ is tied to changes in the strength of religious identity among US adults and, in turn, how the rise of the nones has affected the relationships between religious identity, religious belief, and religious behavior. In particular, we show that, as the percentage of US adults who do not identify with a religion has grown, the correlations between religious identification, belief, and behavior have increased. In short, the rise of the nones has led to more congruence between measures of religion.  相似文献   
82.
83.
84.
85.
The current study examines people's perceptions of a male anabolic steroid (AS) user. Differences in perceptions on the Steroid Perception Questionnaire (SPQ; Schwerin & Corcoran, 1992) were measured. Subjects (N= 399) received 1 of 5 slide presentations: an athletically active individual, a drug-free, an AS-using, a cocaine-using, and a cheating bodybuilder. Results show the AS-using protagonist significantly different (more negatively rated) than his drug-free counterparts and similar to the cocaine-using bodybuilder (p < .05). Differences among presentation protagonists on SPQ dimensions and comparisons to the Schwerin and Corcoran (1992) written-mode findings are examined further.  相似文献   
86.
Following the study of Gibson & Curran (1974), a further sample of 45 subjects were tested on the Eysenck Personality Inventory (EPI) and a slightly modified form of the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale (SHSS) in precisely the same way. The results in this second sample were broadly the same as those obtained in the earlier study. Combining the two samples, it was found that the sex variable provided some interesting contrasts. The power of the lie scale of the EPI to predict hypnotic susceptibility observed earlier was found to be a significant effect only for males. While there was no significant difference between the sexes in terms of the means and S.D.S. of the extraversion (E) and neuroticism (N) scales, when the interaction of these scales was studied males and females differed significantly. The population from the two studies (n = 88) was analysed by means of polar coordinates in the manner suggested by Eysenck (1966) with regard to the E nad N scales. Esyenck's prediction as to hypnotic susceptibility was strikingly confirmed. These data are briefly discussed in terms of alternative approaches to hypnosis from the 'state' and the 'non-state' viewpoints.  相似文献   
87.
The purpose of the experiments and analyses was to discover whether the “independence” model, which successfully accounts for the perception of synthetic multi-dimensional stimuli, would also describe the perception of speech in noise. Two sets of four words were chosen which could be considered phonetically to be describable in terms of two dimensions. The S/R matrix was analysed as if the stimuli were bi-dimensional elementary stimuli. Satisfactory fits were obtained by the independence model. Two analyses were conducted upon data from Miller and Nicely (1955) and Conrad (1964) to discover whether the perception of phonemes was also predictable on the independence model. In general this was found to be the case.  相似文献   
88.
Two experiments are reported in which human performance was compared under continuous 90 dB. white noise, after loss of sleep, under a combination of the two and under appropriate control conditions. The results suggest that noise is effective in reducing the deterioration in performance, characteristic of loss of sleep. The relevance of the result to the arousal theory of loss of sleep is discussed.  相似文献   
89.
Porntida Tanjitpiyanond  Jolanda Jetten  Kim Peters  Ashwini Ashokkumar  Oumar Barry  Matthew Billet  Maja Becker  Robert W. Booth  Diego Castro  Juana Chinchilla  Giulio Costantini  Egon Dejonckheere  Girts Dimdins  Yasemin Erbas  Agustín Espinosa  Gillian Finchilescu  Ángel Gómez  Roberto González  Nobuhiko Goto  Aya Hatano  Lea Hartwich  Somboon Jarukasemthawee  Jaya Kumar Karunagharan  Lindsay M. Novak  Jinseok P. Kim  Michal Kohút  Yi Liu  Steve Loughnan  Ike E. Onyishi  Charity N. Onyishi  Micaela Varela  Iris S. Pattara-angkoon  Müjde Peker  Kullaya Pisitsungkagarn  Muhammad Rizwan  Eunkook M. Suh  William Swann  Eddie M. W. Tong  Rhiannon N. Turner  Niels Vanhasbroeck  Paul A. M. Van Lange  Christin-Melanie Vauclair  Alexander Vinogradov  Grace Wacera  Zhechen Wang  Susilo Wibisono  Victoria Wai-Lan Yeung 《European journal of social psychology》2023,53(2):367-382
There is a growing body of work suggesting that social class stereotypes are amplified when people perceive higher levels of economic inequality—that is, the wealthy are perceived as more competent and assertive and the poor as more incompetent and unassertive. The present study tested this prediction in 32 societies and also examines the role of wealth-based categorization in explaining this relationship. We found that people who perceived higher economic inequality were indeed more likely to consider wealth as a meaningful basis for categorization. Unexpectedly, however, higher levels of perceived inequality were associated with perceiving the wealthy as less competent and assertive and the poor as more competent and assertive. Unpacking this further, exploratory analyses showed that the observed tendency to stereotype the wealthy negatively only emerged in societies with lower social mobility and democracy and higher corruption. This points to the importance of understanding how socio-structural features that co-occur with economic inequality may shape perceptions of the wealthy and the poor.  相似文献   
90.
Perceptions of individuals and groups are inextricably intertwined. An individual is perceived differently depending on the group he or she belongs to, and the perception of the group depends on the characteristics of single group members that are encountered or come to mind. However, this mutual influence is far from uniform. A review of the literature indicates that sometimes the individual is perceived as similar to a group, and sometimes as dissimilar. Likewise, the group can be either assimilated toward or contrasted away from the individual. In the present article, we propose that these diverse effects may be explained by a comparison process between the individual and the group. More specifically, we argue that the comparative thinking style during this comparison (i.e. whether one focuses on similarities or dissimilarities) shapes the direction of the influence.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号