首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   354篇
  免费   5篇
  359篇
  2020年   6篇
  2019年   7篇
  2018年   8篇
  2016年   4篇
  2015年   4篇
  2014年   7篇
  2013年   57篇
  2012年   6篇
  2011年   11篇
  2010年   9篇
  2008年   6篇
  2007年   6篇
  2006年   9篇
  2005年   6篇
  2004年   9篇
  2003年   4篇
  2002年   11篇
  2000年   7篇
  1999年   6篇
  1998年   8篇
  1997年   4篇
  1996年   6篇
  1995年   3篇
  1994年   6篇
  1993年   3篇
  1992年   6篇
  1991年   10篇
  1990年   4篇
  1989年   6篇
  1988年   6篇
  1987年   3篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   5篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   5篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   5篇
  1978年   4篇
  1977年   3篇
  1976年   3篇
  1975年   3篇
  1974年   8篇
  1973年   8篇
  1972年   7篇
  1971年   5篇
  1970年   5篇
  1968年   5篇
  1966年   4篇
  1965年   3篇
排序方式: 共有359条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
These two very different but complementary discussions address two different “registers” of my thinking in my paper on patients who “live in airless intrapsychic worlds.” Harris is most interested in the meta-theoretical and historical contexts and frames of my theory and clinical approach, viewing my contribution as an example of various broad trends in contemporary relational psychoanalytic thought. Shaw, by contrast, engages with my particular insights and clinical/theoretical framework for understanding and working with patients whose struggles very much resemble and overlap with the struggles of the adult children of “traumatically narcissistic” parents, which his seminal work has focused on. In my Replies I voice my appreciation of each of their unique perspectives on my ideas, and engage with each of them regarding certain differences in our theoretical and clinical sensibilities.  相似文献   
52.
I discuss the relatedness with certain hard-to-engage patients as a kind of dissociative enactment, and I present a case illustration of a man whom I liked but could not help.  相似文献   
53.
Summaiy: Three groups of subjects rated each item in Rotter's I-E Scale for its social desirability value. Social desirability scale values (SDSV) of paired items were compared with one another. Results indicated that paired items are not sufficiently similar in their SDSVs, thus enabling subjects to respond on the basis of social desirability. All three groups rated internal items as being significantly more socially desirable than external items. The correlations between the SDSV ratings of the three groups were highly significant. The results indicate that the scale, as it is currently used, is seriously deficient.  相似文献   
54.
Clinical experience involving the treatment of patients with comorbid borderline and narcissistic personality disorders suggests that this patient population is among the more difficult to treat within the personality disorder spectrum. In this article, we present refinements of Transference Focused Psychotherapy (TFP) based on our clinical experience with and research data on patients with comorbid narcissistic personality disorder/borderline personality disorder (NPD/BPD). We briefly review object relations formulations of severe narcissistic pathology, as well as recent research in attachment and the allied concept of mentalization, which have provided a new lens through which to view narcissistic disorders. The research findings from two randomized clinical trials demonstrating the efficacy and effectiveness of TFP are presented. The data from the two Randomized Clinical Trials (RCT) allowed for the study of the characteristics of the subgroup of borderline personality disorder patients who have comorbid NPD/BPD. Findings on comorbidity, attachment status, capacity for mentalization, and level of personality organization of borderline patients with comorbid NPD/BPD, compared with borderline patients without comorbid narcissistic pathology (BPD), are presented. Clinical implications of the observed group differences are discussed, with a focus on refinements in the technique of TFP. Clinical case material is presented to illustrate the specific challenges posed by narcissistic patients to carrying out TFP in each phase of treatment.  相似文献   
55.
56.
ABSTRACT

We investigated the feasibility of using the Space Fortress (SF) game, a complex video game originally developed to study complex skill acquisition in young adults, to improve executive control processes in cognitively healthy older adults. The study protocol consisted of 36 one-hour game play sessions over 3 months with cognitive evaluations before and after, and a follow-up evaluation at 6 months. Sixty participants were randomized to one of three conditions: Emphasis Change (EC) – elders were instructed to concentrate on playing the entire game but place particular emphasis on a specific aspect of game play in each particular game; Active Control (AC) – game play with standard instructions; Passive Control (PC) – evaluation sessions without game play. Primary outcome measures were obtained from five tasks, presumably tapping executive control processes. A total of 54 older adults completed the study protocol. One measure of executive control, WAIS-III letter–number sequencing, showed improvement in performance from pre- to post-evaluations in the EC condition, but not in the other two conditions. These initial findings are modest but encouraging. Future SF interventions need to carefully consider increasing the duration and or the intensity of the intervention by providing at-home game training, reducing the motor demands of the game, and selecting appropriate outcome measures.  相似文献   
57.
ABSTRACT

The hypercorrection effect, which refers to the finding that errors committed with high confidence are more likely to be corrected than are low confidence errors, has been replicated many times, and with both young adults and children. In the present study, we contrasted older with younger adults. Participants answered general-information questions, made confidence ratings about their answers, were given corrective feedback, and then were retested on questions that they had gotten wrong. While younger adults showed the hypercorrection effect, older adults, despite higher overall accuracy on the general-information questions and excellent basic metacognitive ability, showed a diminished hypercorrection effect. Indeed, the correspondence between their confidence in their errors and the probability of correction was not significantly greater than zero, showing, for the first time, that a particular participant population is selectively impaired on this error correction task. These results potentially offer leverage both on the mechanisms underlying the hypercorrection effect and on reasons for older adults’ memory impairments, as well as on memory functions that are spared.  相似文献   
58.
信任是医患关系的一个基本特征,是社会和医生之间的隐性合同的一部分。合同要求双方的期望值。病人会告诉医生他们内心深处的个人隐私,信任医生会为他们保守秘密,允许医生在他们生病和悲伤时进入家里和医院。但是,要做到这些,医生必须维持包括关爱、保密和承诺的伦理标准。因为信任就像个很小但却很珍贵的宝石-很难得到,又很容易失去,失去之后想要在找回来极为困难。  相似文献   
59.
This paper suggests that the nature of leadership as a significant human characteristic, derived from both philosophical and psychological theorizing, can appropriately be seen as egalitarian (in Macmurray's sense) and magnanimous (in Aristotle's sense), whilst remaining undemocratic. Empirical research carried out in schools supports the idea that, in particular circumstances, this model of leadership may indeed already be recognized in schools. In addition, there are brief references to the psychology of leadership, to the ways in which learning in schools might be theorized, and to the nature of schools as communities. Finally, implications for 'followers' as well as leaders, and for spiritual development in schools, are explored.  相似文献   
60.
Coding and recoding of hypothesis information was studied in children in Grades 3, 5, and 7, and college students. Each S was tested on several two-choice discrimination problems. A problem consisted of two trials, a training trial and a test trial. The information available for coding on the first trial was varied in three series of problems by using pairs of stimuli differing in 3, 4, or 6 dimensions. Recoding after an error was facilitated in half of the problems in each series by allowing the stimuli to persist briefly after response and feedback. The stimuli terminated immediately after response and feedback in the remaining problems. The second trial of a problem, in which the stimuli differed on only one dimension, was used to probe for the information coded or recoded on the first trial. On the average, fewer dimensions were coded per trial by the younger Ss than by the older Ss. Neither the effects of negative feedback nor the effects of stimulus persistence were found to be related to developmental level. It was concluded that a deficiency in young children's ability to code, but not recode, hypothesis information contributes to their less efficient performance in discrimination problems.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号