全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2222篇 |
免费 | 81篇 |
国内免费 | 15篇 |
专业分类
2318篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 16篇 |
2023年 | 35篇 |
2022年 | 13篇 |
2021年 | 47篇 |
2020年 | 50篇 |
2019年 | 38篇 |
2018年 | 88篇 |
2017年 | 94篇 |
2016年 | 70篇 |
2015年 | 62篇 |
2014年 | 90篇 |
2013年 | 335篇 |
2012年 | 158篇 |
2011年 | 128篇 |
2010年 | 95篇 |
2009年 | 74篇 |
2008年 | 135篇 |
2007年 | 144篇 |
2006年 | 99篇 |
2005年 | 79篇 |
2004年 | 48篇 |
2003年 | 54篇 |
2002年 | 52篇 |
2001年 | 15篇 |
2000年 | 33篇 |
1999年 | 25篇 |
1998年 | 19篇 |
1997年 | 13篇 |
1996年 | 14篇 |
1995年 | 11篇 |
1994年 | 13篇 |
1993年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 14篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1988年 | 9篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 10篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1971年 | 5篇 |
1965年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有2318条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
121.
This study examined to what extent bullying behavior of popular adolescents is responsible for whether bullying is more or
less likely to be accepted or rejected by peers (popularity-norm effect) rather than the behavior of all peers (class norm).
Specifically, the mean level of bullying by the whole class (class norm) was split into behavior of popular adolescents (popularity-norm)
and behavior of non-popular adolescents (non-popularity-norm), and examined in its interaction with individual bullying on
peer acceptance and peer rejection. The data stem from a peer-nominations subsample of TRAILS, a large population-based sample
of adolescent boys and girls (N = 3312). The findings of multilevel regression analyses demonstrated that the negative impact of individual bullying on peer
acceptance and the positive impact on peer rejection were particularly weakened by bullying by popular adolescents. These
results place the class-norm effects found in previous person-group dissimilarity studies in a different light, suggesting
that particularly bullying by popular adolescents is related to the social status attached to bullying.
相似文献
Jan Kornelis DijkstraEmail: |
122.
123.
Alice J. O’Toole Kenneth A. Deffenbacher Dominique Valentin Karen McKee David Huff Hervé Abdi 《Memory & cognition》1998,26(1):146-160
The perception of face gender was examined in the context of extending “face space” models of human face representations to include the perceptual categories defined by male and female faces. We collected data on the recognizability, gender classifiability (reaction time to classify a face as male/female), attractiveness, and masculinity/femininity of individual male and female faces. Factor analyses applied separately to the data for male and female faces yielded the following results. First, for both male and female faces, the recognizability and gender classifiability of faces were independent—a result inconsistent with the hypothesis that both recognizability and gender classifiability depend on a face’s “distance” from the subcategory gender prototype. Instead, caricatured aspects of gender (femininity/masculinity ratings) related to the gender classifiability of the faces. Second, facial attractiveness related inversely to face recognizability for male, but not for female, faces—a result that resolves inconsistencies in previous studies. Third, attractiveness and femininity for female faces were nearly equivalent, but attractiveness and masculinity for male faces were not equivalent. Finally, we applied principal component analysis to the pixel-coded face images with the aim of extracting measures related to the gender classifiability and recognizability of individual faces. We incorporated these model-derived measures into the factor analysis with the human rating and performance measures. 相似文献
124.
Ricker C Lagos V Feldman N Hiyama S Fuentes S Kumar V Gonzalez K Palomares M Blazer K Lowstuter K MacDonald D Weitzel J 《Journal of genetic counseling》2006,15(6):505-514
Background: Cancer genetic counseling and testing is a standard of care option for appropriate families and can identify individuals at increased risk prior to diagnosis, when prevention or detection strategies are most effective. Despite documented efficacy of cancer risk reduction in high-risk individuals, underserved and minority individuals have a disproportionate cancer burden and limited access to genetic counseling. Methods: A needs assessment survey documented gaps in knowledge and interest in prevention. Satellite clinics were established at two indigent healthcare systems. Cancer genetics CME lectures were conducted and referral guidelines disseminated to clinicians who referred patients for counseling. Results: An increase in clinician knowledge was demonstrated post-CME and reflected by quality referrals. Eighty-eight percent of patients kept their appointments. In the predominantly Latina6 (n=77) clinic population, 71.4% were affected with cancer, and 17 mutation positive families were identified. Preliminary data shows a positive impact on patients' motivation and behavior. The majority has expressed satisfaction and reduction in anxiety. Conclusions: This study demonstrates feasibility and acceptability of cancer genetics services in this population, suggesting the potential to reduce cancer morbidity in underserved, high-risk families.“Latino” is the most common census term for individuals of Spanish, Mexican, Central and South American, Cuban, or Puerto Rican descent, referring to ethnicity, and is generally considered a more ethnically/culturally based term for individuals of the aforementioned groups. As this cohort is female the feminine noun “Latina” is used 相似文献
125.
Schmajuk NA Larrauri JA 《Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes》2006,32(1):1-20
Several studies have recently challenged the accuracy of traditional models of classical conditioning that account for some experimental data in terms of a storage deficit. Among other results, it has been reported that extinction of the blocking or overshadowing stimulus results in the recovery of the response to the blocked or overshadowed stimulus, backward blocking shows spontaneous recovery, extinction of the training context results in the recovery from latent inhibition, interposing a delay between conditioning and testing in latent inhibition increases latent inhibition, and latent inhibition antagonizes overshadowing. An existing neural network model of classical conditioning (N. A. Schmajuk, Y. Lam, & J. A. Gray, 1996), which includes an attentional mechanism controlling both storage and retrieval of associations, is able to quantitatively describe these results. 相似文献
126.
Bernard Baumberger André Delorme Jacques Bergeron Martin Paquette Michelangelo Flückiger 《Ecological Psychology》2013,25(3):201-213
Illusory depth perception experienced in driving simulators is afforded by monocular depth information contained in visual displays. Presumably binocular convergence and binocular disparity, though useful for depth perception in real environments, may poorly contribute to illusory depth in a driving simulator. Instead, they may generate conflicting information by revealing the distance of the display screen and its flatness. Nevertheless, illusory depth induced by monocular information contained in visual displays usually produces enough immersion and realism to create the illusion of driving in a real environment. Many authors have noted improved depth perception in paintings, photographs, and even in drawings when viewed monocularly. However, this effect, known as monocular advantage, has never been explored in driving simulation. The purpose of this experiment was to assess whether the effect might exist in driving simulation. It was expected that drivers would perceive distances in depth better and more accurately with a monocular than with a binocular viewing of the display. Distance estimates were evaluated for two types of driving maneuvers referred to as alignment and bisection. Results showed that when significant performance differences between monocular and binocular viewing conditions occurred, target cars were perceived farther in depth and more accurately using monocular vision. Alternative viewing conditions using both eyes are discussed at the end of the article. 相似文献
127.
This article proposes the Charcot effect, in which clinicians describe what they themselves prescribe. It is argued that the Charcot effect can be a critical instrument for exposing how mental illnesses are invented in the process of developing diagnostic systems and conducting psychopharmacological research. We argue that the Charcot effect helps explain the expansion of depression to epidemic proportions, the promotion of social phobia as a pharmaceutical marketing strategy, the profile of panic disorder according to the available medication, and the worse prognosis of schizophrenia in developed countries than in developing countries. Having undertaken this review, we situate the Charcot effect in relation to constructivist psychology. 相似文献
128.
This study explored the association between comprehensive psychological well-being and the metabolic syndrome (MS), as well as whether women without MS markers differ significantly from women with MS (three or more markers) on specific facets of psychological well-being. African (N=102) and Caucasian (N=115) women completed self-report questionnaires on psychological well-being, and biological markers for MS were determined. Results indicated a significant association between comprehensive psychological well-being and the MS in Caucasian women, but not in the case of African women. Caucasian women without MS had higher levels of perceived support from friends and family, environmental mastery, purpose in life, sense of coherence, affect balance and positive relationships than those with MS. African women without MS had higher levels of positive relationships and an optimistic explanatory style, but lower levels of perceived support from family. Further research is indicated. 相似文献
129.
The nature of quality of life in residential care facilities: the case of White older South Africans
Lizanlé van Biljon 《Journal of Psychology in Africa》2013,23(3):201-207
This study describes the nature of quality of life (QoL) from the perspective of older South Africans (n?=?41) in residential care facilities (female =?75%; age range 62–95 years). The residents participated in individual in-depth interviews and focus groups. They also set down their narrative reflections on their QoL in journals. Data were analysed by means of interpretative phenomenological analysis. Findings reveal the resident older South Africans regard QoL as a spiritually informed worldview of life events, coping with challenges and being mindful of others. The residents perceived QoL to include proximity and quality and reciprocity with others. QoL among older people is context and people specific. 相似文献
130.