首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   602篇
  免费   10篇
  2019年   10篇
  2018年   7篇
  2017年   6篇
  2016年   10篇
  2015年   8篇
  2014年   7篇
  2013年   37篇
  2012年   14篇
  2011年   22篇
  2010年   12篇
  2009年   6篇
  2008年   14篇
  2007年   20篇
  2006年   41篇
  2005年   21篇
  2004年   6篇
  2003年   24篇
  2002年   19篇
  2001年   21篇
  2000年   19篇
  1999年   17篇
  1998年   7篇
  1997年   9篇
  1996年   10篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   10篇
  1993年   7篇
  1992年   14篇
  1991年   13篇
  1990年   5篇
  1989年   11篇
  1988年   17篇
  1987年   7篇
  1986年   10篇
  1985年   7篇
  1984年   7篇
  1983年   7篇
  1982年   9篇
  1980年   6篇
  1979年   6篇
  1976年   7篇
  1975年   6篇
  1974年   7篇
  1973年   9篇
  1971年   8篇
  1970年   12篇
  1969年   7篇
  1968年   8篇
  1967年   6篇
  1966年   5篇
排序方式: 共有612条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
111.
112.
113.
Subjects judged whether the proposed product for two multipliers was true or false. Each equation could be judged as plausible or implausible because a product must be even if any of its multipliers is even; otherwise, it must be odd. A proposed product that violates the required odd-even status of the product—that is, deviates from the correct product, whether odd or even, by an odd value (e.g., splits of ±l, ±3, ±5)—can be rejected as false before normal processing is completed (i.e., before the correct product is retrieved and compared with the proposed product). Subjects were indeed faster and more accurate in rejecting a split of +1 or +3 than a split of +2 or +4, and this effect increased as the number of even multipliers in the pair increased. Subjects did not use the odd-even rule when either multiplier was 0 or 1 (Experiment 2), perhaps because other rules are available to bypass normal processing in those cases. A similar odd-even rule is used in sum verification (Krueger &Hallford), and use of the odd-even rules may help to explain why oddness and evenness are such salient features of numbers as abstract concepts (Shepard, Kilpatric, & Cunningham).  相似文献   
114.
A person's weight may be perceived as an important aspect of his or her sexuality and a significant determinant of his or her interpersonal sexual experiences. However, researchers interested in body weight and sexuality have focused exclusively on sexual disorders found in individuals with eating disorders; consequently, little is known about people's beliefs about weight and sexuality, despite the individual and interpersonal significance of such beliefs. Undergraduates received information about a male or female, obese or normal-weight stimulus person and then evaluated that person along several dimensions related to sexuality. Participants believed that an obese man's sexual experiences would be highly similar to those of a normal-weight man. However, participants viewed an obese woman as less sexually attractive, skilled, warm, and responsive, and perceived her as less likely to experience desire and various sexual behaviors than a normal-weight woman. In addition, participants believed that an obese woman was less sexually attractive, skilled, warm, and responsive than an obese man.  相似文献   
115.
116.
Acquisition and retention of a list of paired associates were measured either by cued recall or by multiple-choice recognition. The method of testing used during original learning was combined factorially with the type of test administered after a 1-week interval. Speed of learning to criterion under the two procedures was comparable. Long-term recognition was substantially higher than recall. This difference did not interact significantly with the method of learning. Recall benefited substantially from a prior test of recognition, but there was little effect when the order of tests was reversed. It is concluded that (a) both occurrence and retrieval information were stored under each condition of learning, and (b) occurrence information is less subject to forgetting than retrieval information.  相似文献   
117.
118.
119.
Five developmental experiments examine the role of context in visual search. Experiments I and II presented either lists or paragraphs that were either in meaningful context or scrambled. Results showed a significant interaction of these variables. Experiment II examined within-item context or predictability and replicated and extended the work of Brand (1971) and Ingling (1972). Experiments IV and V examined search through lists of various widths; results showed a significant interaction of Grade × Position × Width reflecting the different perceptual spans of children and adults. Although these were search tasks, appropriate comparisons between list and paragraph performance were made allowing interpretations of the results to be supportive of a model of reading in which meaningfulness, predictability, and peripheral vision play critical roles.  相似文献   
120.
The present study attempted to eliminate the word superiority effect found in letter search by holding the target letter fixed across trials. The expectation was that the target would thereby become so familiar and salient that the subject would "see" only that letter during search. Even with the target-letter held fixed (Experiment I), however, search was still faster through words than through nonwords, indicating that nontarget letters had been "seen" as well. Search also remained faster through words than through nonwords when the number of exposures to the target was further increased by having the subject search for the absence rather than the presence of the target letter (Experiment III). In line with the notion of "proofreader's errors," however, search became relatively more accurate on nonwords than on words when it required detection of the "mutilation" produced by substituting an F for an E, e.g., BASKFT, BAKFRY (Experiment IV).  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号