全文获取类型
收费全文 | 711篇 |
免费 | 34篇 |
专业分类
745篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 5篇 |
2023年 | 11篇 |
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 17篇 |
2019年 | 14篇 |
2018年 | 22篇 |
2017年 | 27篇 |
2016年 | 30篇 |
2015年 | 15篇 |
2014年 | 16篇 |
2013年 | 86篇 |
2012年 | 31篇 |
2011年 | 30篇 |
2010年 | 16篇 |
2009年 | 28篇 |
2008年 | 34篇 |
2007年 | 29篇 |
2006年 | 35篇 |
2005年 | 26篇 |
2004年 | 19篇 |
2003年 | 26篇 |
2002年 | 21篇 |
2001年 | 14篇 |
2000年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 16篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 8篇 |
1993年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 6篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 8篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 5篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
1972年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 8篇 |
1969年 | 3篇 |
1966年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有745条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
31.
32.
Davis CJ 《Behavior research methods》2005,37(1):65-70
This article describes a Windows program that enables users to obtain a broad range of statistics concerning the properties
of word and nonword stimuli, including measures of word frequency, orthographic similarity, orthographic and phonological
structure, age of acquisition, and imageability. It is designed for use by researchers in psycholinguistics, particularly
those concerned with recognition of isolated words. The program computes measures of orthographic similarity on line, either
with respect to a default vocabulary of 30,605 words or to a vocabulary specified by the user. In addition to providing standard
orthographic neighborhood measures, the program can be used to obtain information about other forms of orthographic similarity,
such as transposed-letter similarity and embedded-word similarity. It is available, free of charge, from the following Web
site: http://www.maccs.mq.edu.au/≈colin/N-Watch/. 相似文献
33.
In a recent study, Pelli (1999 Science 285 844-846) performed a set of perceptual experiments using portrait paintings by Chuck Close. Close's work is similar to the 'Lincoln' portraits of Harmon and Julesz (1973 Science 180 1194-1197) in that they are composite images consisting of coarsely sampled, individually painted, mostly homogeneous cells. Pelli showed that perceived shape was dependent on size, refuting findings that perception of this type is scale-invariant. In an attempt to broaden this finding we designed a series of experiments to investigate the interaction of 2-D scale and 3-D structure on our perception of 3-D shape. We present a series of experiments where field of view, 3-D object complexity, 2-D image resolution, viewing orientation, and subject matter of the stimulus are manipulated. On each trial, observers indicated if the depicted objects appeared to be 2-D or 3-D. Results for face stimuli are similar to Pelli's, while more geometrically complex stimuli show a further interaction of the 3-D information with distance and image information. Complex objects need more image information to be seen as 3-D when close; however, as they are moved further away from the observer, there is a bias for seeing them as 3-D objects rather than 2-D images. Finally, image orientation, relative to the observer, shows little effect, suggesting the participation of higher-level processes in the determination of the 'solidness' of the depicted object. Thus, we show that the critical image resolution depends systematically on the geometric complexity of the object depicted. 相似文献
34.
Put your money where your mouth is! Explaining collective action tendencies through group-based anger and group efficacy 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
van Zomeren M Spears R Fischer AH Leach CW 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2004,87(5):649-664
Insights from appraisal theories of emotion are used to integrate elements of theories on collective action. Three experiments with disadvantaged groups systematically manipulated procedural fairness (Study 1), emotional social support (Study 2), and instrumental social support (Study 3) to examine their effects on collective action tendencies through group-based anger and group efficacy. Results of structural equation modeling showed that procedural fairness and emotional social support affected the group-based anger pathway (reflecting emotion-focused coping), whereas instrumental social support affected the group efficacy pathway (reflecting problem-focused coping), constituting 2 distinct pathways to collective action tendencies. Analyses of the means suggest that collective action tendencies become stronger the more fellow group members "put their money where their mouth is." The authors discuss how their dual pathway model integrates and extends elements of current approaches to collective action. 相似文献
35.
36.
In two studies, the authors investigated guilt as a response to group-based advantage. Consistent with its conceptualization as a self-focused emotion, White guilt was based in self-focused beliefs in racial inequality. Thus, guilt was associated with belief in White privilege (Study 1) and resulted from seeing European Americans as perpetrators of racial discrimination (Study 2). Just as personal guilt is associated with efforts at restitution, White guilt was predictive of support for affirmative action programs aimed at compensating African Americans. White guilt was not, however, predictive of support for noncompensatory efforts at promoting equality, such as affirmative action programs that increase opportunities (Study 2). In contrast, the other-focused emotion of group-based sympathy was a more general predictor of support for different affirmative action policies. Our findings demonstrate the benefits and limits of group-based guilt as a basis of support for social equality and highlight the value of understanding the specific emotions elicited in intergroup contexts. 相似文献
37.
Raz, Shapiro, Fan, and Posner (2002) have recently provided a compelling demonstration of enhanced attentional control under post-hypnotic suggestion. Using the classic color-word interference paradigm, in which the task is to ignore a word and to name the color in which it is printed (e.g., RED in green, say "green"), they gave a post-hypnotic instruction to participants that they would be unable to read. This eliminated Stroop interference in high suggestibility participants but did not alter interference in low suggestibility participants. replicated this pattern and further demonstrated that it is not due to a visual strategy (such as blurring or looking at a different location). As a historical footnote, we describe a "case study" from 18 years ago in which we observed the same result using a hypnotic instruction to a single highly suggestible individual that he could not read. The elimination of Stroop interference has important implications for both the study of attention and the study of hypnosis. 相似文献
38.
39.
Bradley C. Courtenay Sharan B. Merriam Patricia M. Reeves 《Journal of religion and health》1999,38(3):203-218
Studies of religious faith consistently document the important role of religion in the lives of adults. Investigations of adult religious faith also suggest that a life crisis can stimulate faith development. However, these earlier studies do not explain how a life-threatening crisis changes adult religious faith. This study examines the impact of an HIV-positive diagnosis on adult religious faith development. Interviews were conducted with 18 HIV-positive men and women under 45 years of age. The researchers found that an HIV-positive diagnosis affects faith development in these ways: the perspective of faith shifts from a religious to a spiritual orientation for which the participants feel a stronger sense of ownership; the concept of God changes from an authoritarian to an empowering figure, and views of the self are transformed to include others. 相似文献
40.